Publication Date
In 2025 | 29 |
Since 2024 | 114 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 336 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 748 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 1525 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Practitioners | 124 |
Teachers | 76 |
Researchers | 75 |
Parents | 22 |
Administrators | 6 |
Policymakers | 4 |
Support Staff | 2 |
Community | 1 |
Students | 1 |
Location
Australia | 65 |
Canada | 58 |
United Kingdom (England) | 40 |
United Kingdom | 37 |
Germany | 31 |
France | 30 |
Italy | 30 |
Netherlands | 29 |
United States | 27 |
China | 26 |
Japan | 22 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Education for All Handicapped… | 1 |
Goals 2000 | 1 |
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
No Child Left Behind Act 2001 | 1 |
United Nations Convention on… | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Does not meet standards | 5 |
Ingersoll, Brooke – Journal of Positive Behavior Interventions, 2011
Naturalistic interventions show promise for improving language in children with autism. Specific interventions differ in direct elicitation of child language and indirect language stimulation, and thus may produce different language outcomes. This study compared the effects of responsive interaction, milieu teaching, and a combined intervention on…
Descriptors: Stimulation, Intervention, Autism, Child Language
LeBarton, Eve Angela Sauer – ProQuest LLC, 2010
Previous investigators have found significant relations between children's early spontaneous gesture and their subsequent vocabulary development: the more gesture children produce early, the larger their later vocabularies. The questions we address here are (1) whether we can increase children's gesturing through experimental manipulation and, if…
Descriptors: Vocabulary Development, Language Acquisition, Nonverbal Communication, Child Language
Bass-Ringdahl, Sandie M. – Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2010
This article investigated the relationship between age at onset of canonical babbling and audibility of amplified speech in children with hearing impairment. Thirteen children with severe-profound hearing impairment and two children with normal hearing participated in a longitudinal investigation of vocalization development. A nonconcurrent…
Descriptors: Hearing Impairments, Assistive Technology, Language Acquisition, Child Language
Huttunen, Kerttu; Valimaa, Taina – Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2010
Our aim was to obtain versatile information on the communication and socioemotional development of implanted children in their everyday environment. We studied 18 children implanted unilaterally at the mean age of 3 years 4 months. All had normal nonverbal intelligence, but 8 (44%) had concomitant problems. Their parents filled out semistructured…
Descriptors: Speech, Social Life, Oral Language, Individual Development
Jorgensen, Rune Norgaard; Dale, Philip S.; Bleses, Dorthe; Fenson, Larry – Journal of Child Language, 2010
Parent report has proven a valid and cost-effective means of evaluating early child language. Norming datasets for these instruments, which provide the basis for standardized comparisons of individual children to a population, can also be used to derive norms for the acquisition of individual words in production and comprehension and also early…
Descriptors: Cost Effectiveness, Child Language, Young Children, Norms
Bernal, Savita; Dehaene-Lambertz, Ghislaine; Millotte, Severine; Christophe, Anne – Developmental Science, 2010
Syntax allows human beings to build an infinite number of new sentences from a finite stock of words. Because toddlers typically utter only one or two words at a time, they have been thought to have no syntax. Using event-related potentials (ERPs), we demonstrated that 2-year-olds do compute syntactic structure when listening to spoken sentences.…
Descriptors: Sentences, Topography, Verbs, Nouns
Whitmarsh, Judith – International Journal of Early Years Education, 2011
An increasing emphasis is being placed on the importance of speech, language and communication (SLC) development during the first two years of life, since this contributes to cognitive ability and to later educational outcomes. This article explores what disadvantaged, first-time mothers know and understand about three key contributors to positive…
Descriptors: Infants, Language Acquisition, Language Skills, Mothers
Bjorn, Piia M.; Kakkuri, Irma; Karvonen, Pirkko; Leppanen, Paavo H. T. – Early Child Development and Care, 2012
This paper reports the outcome of a multi-sensory intervention on infant language skills. A programme titled "Rhyming Game and Exercise Club", which included kinaesthetic-tactile mother-child rhyming games performed in natural joint attention situations, was intended to accelerate Finnish six- to eight-month-old infants' language development. The…
Descriptors: Intervention, Expressive Language, Receptive Language, Stimulation
Notley, Anna; Zhou, Peng; Crain, Stephen; Thornton, Rosalind – Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics, 2009
Children often produce nonadult responses to sentences with the focus operator only, such as "Only the cat is holding a flag." For example, children often accept this sentence as a description of a situation in which a cat holds a flag and a duck holds both a flag and a balloon. One proposed analysis, by Paterson, Liversedge, Rowland & Filik…
Descriptors: Sentences, Form Classes (Languages), Mandarin Chinese, Child Language
Kooijman, Valesca; Hagoort, Peter; Cutler, Anne – Infancy, 2009
Recognizing word boundaries in continuous speech requires detailed knowledge of the native language. In the first year of life, infants acquire considerable word segmentation abilities. Infants at this early stage in word segmentation rely to a large extent on the metrical pattern of their native language, at least in stress-based languages. In…
Descriptors: Cues, Infants, Indo European Languages, Language Acquisition
Nilsen, Elizabeth S.; Graham, Susan A.; Pettigrew, Tamara – Journal of Child Language, 2009
We assessed the effect of specificity of speaker information about an object on three-year-olds' word mappings. When children heard a novel label followed by specific information about an object at exposure, children subsequently mapped the label to that object at test. When children heard only specific information about an object at exposure,…
Descriptors: Word Recognition, Vocabulary Development, Cognitive Mapping, Child Language
Rispoli, Matthew; Hadley, Pamela A.; Holt, Janet K. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2009
Purpose: This study tests empirical predictions of a maturational model for the growth of tense in children younger than 36 months using a type-based productivity measure. Method: Caregiver-child language samples were collected from 20 typically developing children every 3 months from 21 to 33 months of age. Growth in the productivity of tense…
Descriptors: Productivity, Investigations, Morphemes, Child Language
Zamuner, Tania S. – Journal of Child Language, 2009
This research examines phonological neighbourhoods in the lexicons of children acquiring English. Analyses of neighbourhood densities were done on children's earliest words and on a corpus of spontaneous speech, used to measure neighbours in the target language. Neighbourhood densities were analyzed for words created by changing segments in…
Descriptors: Speech, Child Language, Auditory Perception, Language Acquisition
Keren-Portnoy, Tamar; Majorano, Marinella; Vihman, Marilyn M. – Journal of Child Language, 2009
This study assesses the extent of phonetic continuity between babble and words in four Italian children followed longitudinally from 0;9 or 0;10 to 2;0--two with relatively rapid and two with slower lexical growth. Prelinguistic phonetic characteristics, including both (a) consistent use of specific consonants and (b) age of onset and extent of…
Descriptors: Phonetics, Italian, Child Language, Infants
Crestani, Catherine-Ann M.; Clendon, Sally A.; Hemsley, Bronwyn – Journal of Intellectual & Developmental Disability, 2010
Background: This study examined the narrative vocabulary of typically developing children for the purpose of guiding vocabulary selection for children with complex communication needs. Method: Eight children in their first year of schooling (aged 5 years 0 months to 5 years 8 months) and 10 children in their second year of schooling (aged 6 years…
Descriptors: Augmentative and Alternative Communication, Child Language, Vocabulary, Children