NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Does not meet standards5
Showing 4,846 to 4,860 of 5,713 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Lein, Laura; Brenneis, Donald – Language in Society, 1978
Focuses on arguments among White American children in a small town in New England, Black American children of migrant harvesters, and rural Hindi-speaking Fiji Indian children. Findings suggest that, while repetition, inversion, and escalation are common to all three cultures, there is considerable variation as to how they are used. (EJS)
Descriptors: Child Language, Comparative Analysis, Cross Cultural Studies, Discourse Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Boggs, Stephen T. – Language in Society, 1978
Describes a pattern of verbal disputing frequently engaged in by children in Hawaii who have some Polynesian ancestry. This pattern, which is characterized by the forceful use of "not!" as an outright contradiction of one speaker by another, is traced from early childhood into adolescence in the context of relationships in which it develops. (EJS)
Descriptors: Adolescents, Age Differences, Child Language, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Hollos, Marida; Beeman, William – Language in Society, 1978
Attempts to demonstrate that there is a "cultural communicative style" operating in the issuance of directives that is distinct for different cultures. Emphasis is placed on investigation of children's strategies in their total communicative behavior, both linguistic and nonlinguistic. (EJS)
Descriptors: Child Development, Child Language, Children, Communication (Thought Transfer)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Wellman, Henry M.; Estes, David – Discourse Processes, 1987
Examines whether young children make explicit references to beliefs, ideas, thoughts, and intentions. Relates that three-year-old children correctly distinguish between real and mental items and that they think and dream about things that are not real. Concludes that conceptual development theories portraying early development as concrete and…
Descriptors: Child Development, Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Structures
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Urzua, Carole – TESOL Quarterly, 1987
A six-month observational study of Southeast Asian children (N=4) as they wrote and revised various pieces in English (their second language) revealed that the subjects developed three areas of writing skill: a sense of audience, a sense of voice, and a sense of power in language. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Child Language, Elementary Education, English (Second Language), Feedback
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Evans, Mary – Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 1987
Describes one aspect of a Welsh/English bilingual child being raised in England. The father is a native speaker of Welsh, and the mother has learned Welsh in order to speak it to her son. The father accommodates both the mother's and the child's linguistic errors. Areas of accommodation are identified and possible reasons discussed. (Author/LMO)
Descriptors: Bilingualism, Child Language, English, Error Analysis (Language)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Smolak, Linda – Journal of Child Language, 1987
An eight-month longitudinal study measuring infants' (N=8) temperament characteristics of activity level, task persistence, and affect and discourse and pragmatic features of their mothers' speech revealed complex interactions between maternal speech and infant temperament. It is argued that nonlinguistic child behaviors may influence maternal…
Descriptors: Attachment Behavior, Child Language, Discourse Analysis, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Schwartz, Richard G.; And Others – Applied Psycholinguistics, 1987
Comparison of language-impaired two- to three-year-olds (N=10) and normal one-year-olds (N=15) matched for expressive language revealed that the language-impaired subjects acquired a greater number of object concepts presented in a no-action condition than the normal children, although language-impaired subjects' extensions of the names to new…
Descriptors: Child Language, Comparative Analysis, Concept Formation, Context Clues
Stough, Con; And Others – Exceptional Child, 1988
Thirty-three congenitally physically disabled children, aged 4-6 and 14-16, were studied in order to define objective means for identifying children exhibiting the Cocktail Party Syndrome. A measure of the irrelevancy of language content was found to reliably distinguish such children from children of similar age and measured IQ. (Author/JDD)
Descriptors: Child Language, Communicative Competence (Languages), Congenital Impairments, Educational Diagnosis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick; And Others – Journal of Child Language, 1987
Three studies assessing language comprehension of infants and toddlers through a method requiring a minimum of motor movement, no speech production, and differential visual fixation of two simultaneously presented video events provide insight into children's emerging linguistic capabilities and help resolve controversies about language production…
Descriptors: Child Language, Correlation, Language Acquisition, Language Aptitude
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Anderson, Raquel; Smith, Bruce L. – Journal of Child Language, 1987
Phonetic and phonological analysis of spontaneous speech of six 2-year-old monolingual Puerto Rican Spanish-learning children revealed several sound usage patterns similar to those found in English and other language-learning children, supporting the claim that certain universal patterns exist in phonological development. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Articulation (Speech), Child Language, Distinctive Features (Language), Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Cutler, Anne; Swinney, David A. – Journal of Child Language, 1987
Studies analyzing children's response time to detect word targets revealed that six-year-olds and younger children generally did not show the response time advantage for accented target words which adult listeners show, providing support for the argument that the processing advantage for accented words reflects the semantic role of accent as an…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Child Language, Correlation, Deep Structure
Reutzel, D. Ray; Hollingsworth, Paul M. – Academic Therapy, 1988
The whole language philosophy of teaching reading and writing is outlined and related to children's acquisition of oral and written language. A typical whole language classroom is described, along with the roles of the teacher and students. A chart contrasts whole language theory with more typical methods of literacy education. (Author/JDD)
Descriptors: Child Language, Classroom Environment, Classroom Techniques, Educational Philosophy
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Tomasello, Michael; Farrar, Michael Jeffrey – Journal of Child Language, 1986
Describes a lexical training program developed to teach object, visible movement, and invisible movement words to children at stage 5 (N=7) and stage 6 (N=16) object permanence development. Stage 6 children learned all three types of words equally well, while stage 5 children learned object and visible movement but not invisible movement words.…
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Structures, Comprehension
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Moss, R. Kay – English Quarterly, 1986
Analyzes the written language learning of kindergarten children as a transaction among writers, teachers, and the writing process and concludes that children, when given the opportunity to take risks with written language, create with semantic intent. (DF)
Descriptors: Child Language, Kindergarten Children, Language Arts, Language Processing
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  320  |  321  |  322  |  323  |  324  |  325  |  326  |  327  |  328  |  ...  |  381