NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Does not meet standards5
Showing 3,616 to 3,630 of 5,713 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Homzie, M. J.; Gravitt, Carol B. – Journal of Child Language, 1977
In retelling 20 stories, 23 nursery-school children often refused to produce sentences in which causation was stated directly, but readily retold causation-implied utterances. Other results are discussed. (Author/RM)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Development, Concept Formation, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Corsaro, William A. – Language in Society, 1977
The analysis of videotaped, naturally occurring, adult-child interaction led to the isolation of the clarification request as a consistent feature of adult interactive styles. The importance of these demands, their nature, how adults deal with them, and their effects on children's communicative development are discussed. (CHK)
Descriptors: Child Development, Child Language, Communication Skills, Discourse Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Clark, Eve V.; Berman, Ruth A. – Journal of Child Language, 1987
Examination of the types of linguistic knowledge that affect three- to nine-year-olds' (N=60) and adults' (N=12) ability to understand and produce novel compounds in Hebrew revealed that comprehension was achieved ahead of production. Knowledge of morphological form had little effect on comprehension, but was crucial to production. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Child Language, Comprehension
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Pleh, Csaba; And Others – Journal of Child Language, 1987
Hungarian-Russian bilingual preschoolers, in general, paid more attention to allomorphy than did monolingual Hungarian or Russian peers in interpreting transitive sentences with varying word orders. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Bilingualism, Child Language, Comparative Analysis, Comprehension
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Goldfield, Beverly A. – Applied Psycholinguistics, 1987
Longitudinal observation of 12 infants, including measures of child behavior and maternal language and child language, revealed that most subjects acquired a balanced distribution of object labels and social-centered words and phrases. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Child Language, Discourse Analysis, Infants, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Schaerlaekens, A.; And Others – Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 1988
An investigation into the linguistic adaptation process of foreign children adopted by Dutch-speaking families in Belgium identified a short, early adaptation period followed by a period of further acquisition, with marked differences between children younger and older than three years. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Acculturation, Adopted Children, Age Differences, Child Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Chaney, Carolyn – Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1988
Identification of correctly produced and misarticulated /w,r,l,j/ was examined in 12 children, aged 3:6-7:5. The children, their parents, and raters were more successful in identifying correctly produced semivowels than misarticulated ones. Both normal children with developmental substitutions and articulation-impaired children demonstrated…
Descriptors: Acoustic Phonetics, Articulation (Speech), Articulation Impairments, Auditory Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Peck, Sabrina – Language Learning, 1987
Compares native Spanish-speaking (N=9) and native English-speaking kindergarten students' use of language learning acts after individual peer tutoring sessions in English. Findings reveal that students appear to acquire English according to their level of general academic achievement. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Child Language, Correlation, English (Second Language), Kindergarten
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Klecan-Aker, Joan S.; Swank, Paul R. – Journal of Communication Disorders, 1988
The study investigated the effectiveness of a pragmatic language function protocol with a sample of 240 normal preschool children, aged 2-5, in a structured setting. The protocol was shown to be used reliably and was sensitive to differences in the mastery of language skills of normal children of different ages. (Author/JDD)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Child Development, Child Language, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
MacLachlan, Barbara G.; Chapman, Robin S. – Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 1988
Communication breakdowns occurring in the speech of seven language learning-disabled children (aged 9-11) were examined in conversation and narration and compared to normal peers. Length of communication unit and rate of communication breakdowns per communication unit were greater in narration than conversation compared to controls. No differences…
Descriptors: Child Language, Communication Disorders, Comparative Analysis, Dialogs (Language)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Pye, Clifton; And Others – Journal of Child Language, 1988
Analysis and comparison of three independent transcriptions of the same speech sample collected from a hearing child with deaf parents resulted in two descriptions of the child's phonological system--one based on a liberal estimate and the other a conservative estimate of the potential error in the transcripts. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Child Language, Comparative Analysis, Deafness, Error Analysis (Language)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Sokolov, Jeffrey L. – Journal of Child Language, 1988
Investigation of the degree to which cue validity predicted the actual strength of grammatical cues as they are used by speakers of Hebrew revealed strong positive correlations between estimated cue validities and actual cue strengths for all but the youngest age groups of speakers. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Adults, Child Language, Children, Context Clues
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Roskos, Kathleen – Reading Teacher, 1988
Reports the results of a six month qualitative study examining the reading and writing behavior in the natural pretend play of eight children four and five years old. Suggests that literacy is involved in two ways: (1) story making using setting, plot and resolution, and (2) displaying a literacy stance or orientation toward reading and…
Descriptors: Child Language, Early Childhood Education, Kindergarten, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Huttenlocher, Janellen; Smiley, Patricia – Cognitive Psychology, 1987
Three types of overgeneral uses of object names by young children were identified. Production data from 10 children were obtained using a standardized method of recording utterance contexts. Results showed that, like adults, children's object categories applied to objects of particular kinds. Most overgeneral uses were attributable to…
Descriptors: Child Language, Concept Formation, Development, Encoding (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Hoek, Dorothy; And Others – Journal of Child Language, 1986
Analysis of a one-year-old's lexical development suggested factors causing overextensions: using known words for more recently acquired or unknown words; expressing incomplete knowledge of defining features of two or more similar meaning words; producing overextensions of preferred words; using phonologically simpler more than difficult words; and…
Descriptors: Case Studies, Child Language, Communication Skills, Diaries
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  238  |  239  |  240  |  241  |  242  |  243  |  244  |  245  |  246  |  ...  |  381