NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 3,061 to 3,075 of 5,420 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Scheuermann, Amy; van Garderen, Delinda – Mathematics Teaching in the Middle School, 2008
Students are often told to use representations to solve mathematics problems, yet it may be a hindrance for many. This article contains an approach, the Graphic Representational Analysis Process, that teachers can use to analyze representations and focus future instruction. (Contains 7 figures.)
Descriptors: Error Patterns, Misconceptions, Mathematics Instruction, Graphs
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Martinez-Castilla, Pastora; Peppe, Sue – Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 2010
Well-documented Romance-Germanic differences in the use of accent in speech to convey information-structure and focus cause problems for the assessment of prosodic skills in populations with clinical disorders. The strategies for assessing the ability to use lexical and contrastive accent in English and Spanish are reviewed, and studies in the…
Descriptors: Speech Communication, Autism, Spanish, English
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bueno Alastuey, Maria Camino – CALICO Journal, 2010
Communicative competence is the ultimate goal of most learners of a second language and intelligible pronunciation a fundamental part of it. Unfortunately, learners often lack the opportunity to explore how intelligible their speech is for different audiences. Our research investigates whether synchronous-voice computer-mediated communication…
Descriptors: Computer Mediated Communication, Audiences, Communicative Competence (Languages), English (Second Language)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Thouesny, Sylvie – CALICO Journal, 2010
In a project-based approach to teaching a foreign language at the university level, students are often required to participate in several task-based writing activities. In doing so, language learners not only write incorrect forms, but also correct forms of the same structures, both of which provide useful information on their strengths and…
Descriptors: French, College Instruction, Case Studies, Language Proficiency
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Salmon, Karen; Yao, Joanna; Berntsen, Oriana; Pipe, Margaret-Ellen – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2007
We investigated the conditions under which preparatory information presented 1 day before a novel event influenced 6-year-olds' recall 1 week later. Children were assigned to one of six experimental conditions. Three conditions involved preparatory information that described the event accurately but differed according to the presence and type of…
Descriptors: Photography, Novels, Recall (Psychology), Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Stemberger, Joseph Paul – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
Overtensing (the use of an inflected form in place of a nonfinite form, e.g. *"didn't broke" for target "didn't break") is common in early syntax. In a ChiLDES-based study of 36 children acquiring English, I examine the effects of phonological and lexical factors. For irregulars, errors are more common with verbs of low frequency and when…
Descriptors: Syntax, Rhyme, Morphemes, Error Patterns
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
van den Bemt, P. M. L. A.; Robertz, R.; de Jong, A. L.; van Roon, E. N.; Leufkens, H. G. M. – Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 2007
Background: Medication errors can result in harm, unless barriers to prevent them are present. Drug administration errors are less likely to be prevented, because they occur in the last stage of the drug distribution process. This is especially the case in non-alert patients, as patients often form the final barrier to prevention of errors.…
Descriptors: Error Patterns, Foreign Countries, Patients, Mental Retardation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Wolfe, Jeremy M.; Horowitz, Todd S.; Van Wert, Michael J.; Kenner, Naomi M.; Place, Skyler S.; Kibbi, Nour – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2007
In visual search tasks, observers look for targets in displays containing distractors. Likelihood that targets will be missed varies with target prevalence, the frequency with which targets are presented across trials. Miss error rates are much higher at low target prevalence (1%-2%) than at high prevalence (50%). Unfortunately, low prevalence is…
Descriptors: Incidence, Feedback (Response), Search Strategies, Visual Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bott, Lewis; Hoffman, Aaron B.; Murphy, Gregory L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2007
Many theories of category learning assume that learning is driven by a need to minimize classification error. When there is no classification error, therefore, learning of individual features should be negligible. The authors tested this hypothesis by conducting three category-learning experiments adapted from an associative learning blocking…
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Classification, Error Patterns, Hypothesis Testing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Compton, Rebecca J.; Carp, Joshua; Chaddock, Laura; Fineman, Stephanie L.; Quandt, Lorna C.; Ratliff, Jeffrey B. – Brain and Cognition, 2007
This study tested the prediction that the error-related negativity (ERN), a physiological measure of error monitoring, would be enhanced in anxious individuals, particularly in conditions with threatening cues. Participants made gender judgments about faces whose expressions were either happy, angry, or neutral. Replicating prior studies, midline…
Descriptors: Cues, Anxiety, Nonverbal Communication, Physiology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Weems, Carl F.; Costa, Natalie M.; Watts, Sarah E.; Taylor, Leslie K.; Cannon, Melinda F. – Behavior Modification, 2007
This study examined the interrelations among negative cognitive errors, anxiety sensitivity, and anxiety control beliefs and explored their unique and specific associations with anxiety symptoms in a community sample of youth. Existing research has suggested that these constructs are related to childhood anxiety disorder symptoms; however,…
Descriptors: Depression (Psychology), Children, Anxiety, Beliefs
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Maharaj, Aneshkumar – South African Journal of Education, 2008
I report on the findings from research and literature on (a) use of symbols in mathematics, (b) algebraic/trigonometric expressions, (c) solving equations, and (d) functions and calculus. From these, some insights and implications for teaching and learning are derived.
Descriptors: Mathematics Instruction, Symbols (Mathematics), Algebra, Trigonometry
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Pruitt, Beth Anne; Cooper, Justin T. – Beyond Behavior, 2008
Many students, with and without disabilities, experience difficulty reaching an appropriate level of reading fluency even though they may attain an appropriate level of reading accuracy. The National Reading Panel (2000) found that students who lack reading fluency often lack reading comprehension also. Other students lack the necessary strategies…
Descriptors: Reading Comprehension, Reading Fluency, Teaching Methods, Reading Strategies
Yee, Ng Kin; Lam, Toh Tin – Journal of Science and Mathematics Education in Southeast Asia, 2008
This paper reports on students' errors in performing integration of rational functions, a topic of calculus in the pre-university mathematics classrooms. Generally the errors could be classified as those due to the students' weak algebraic concepts and their lack of understanding of the concept of integration. With the students' inability to link…
Descriptors: Calculus, Misconceptions, Mathematics Instruction, College Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kidd, Evan; Lum, Jarrad A. G. – Developmental Science, 2008
Hartshorne and Ullman (2006 ) presented naturalistic language data from 25 children (15 boys, 10 girls) and showed that girls produced more past tense overregularization errors than did boys. In particular, girls were more likely to overregularize irregular verbs whose stems share phonological similarities with regular verbs. It was argued that…
Descriptors: Females, Verbs, Gender Differences, Males
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  201  |  202  |  203  |  204  |  205  |  206  |  207  |  208  |  209  |  ...  |  362