NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Laws, Policies, & Programs
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 151 to 165 of 280 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Silverstein, A. B.; And Others – American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1982
Scales for object permanence and spatial relationships were administered to 98 severely and profoundly mentally retarded children (mean age 13 years) on three occasions, 6 months apart. Differences in the difficulty of the items were quite stable, but their order of difficulty differed appreciably from that for nonretarded infants. (Author/SB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Developmental Stages, Object Permanence, Severe Mental Retardation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
DeLoache, Judy S.; Burns, Nancy M. – Cognition, 1994
Twenty-four- and 30-month-old children were presented with a picture that showed the location of a hidden toy and were then asked to find the toy. The 30-month olds, but not the 24-month-olds, were successful in retrieving the toy. Concludes that 24-month olds did not interpret the pictures as representations of reality. (BC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Object Permanence, Pictorial Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Bai, Dina L.; Bertenthal, Bennett I. – Child Development, 1992
Investigated the possibility that previous reports of a relation between locomotor status and stage-4 object permanence performance could be generalized to performance on an object localization task. Findings suggest that the effects of locomotor experience on infants' search performance are quite specific and mediated by a variety of factors that…
Descriptors: Child Development, Infant Behavior, Infants, Object Permanence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rose, S.A.; Feldman, J.F.; Jankowski, J.J. – Intelligence, 2005
The present study explored the dimensionality of cognition at 12 months by factor analyzing data from a large cohort of preterm and full-term infants (N=182). Two analyses were done. In the first, using only measures used earlier, when the infants were 7 months of age, the same three factors emerged at 12 months as at the earlier age-namely,…
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Recognition (Psychology), Reaction Time, Object Permanence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lleras, Alejandro; Enns, James T. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2004
The negative compatibility effect (NCE) is the surprising result that visual targets that follow a brief prime stimulus and a mask can be identified more rapidly when they are opposite rather than identical to the prime. In a recent article in this journal, S. T. Klapp and L. B. Hinkley (2002) proposed that this reflected a competition between…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Psychological Patterns, Psychological Studies, Inhibition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Merriman, William E.; Evey, Julie A. – Child Development, 2005
If after teaching a label for 1 object, a speaker does not name a nearby object, 3-year-olds tend to reject the label for the nearby object (W.E. Merriman, J.M. Marazita, L.H. Jarvis, J.A. Evey-Burkey, and M. Biggins, 1995a). In Studies 1 (5-year-olds) and 3 (3-year-olds), this effect depended on object similarity. In Study 2, when a speaker used…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Instruction, Age Differences, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Iachini, Tina; Sergi, Ida; Ruggiero, Gennaro; Gnisci, Augusto – Brain and Cognition, 2005
In this preliminary study we investigate gender differences in object location memory. Our purpose is to extend the results about object location memory obtained in laboratory settings to a real 3-D environment and to further distinguish the specific components involved in this kind of memory by considering the strategies adopted to perform the…
Descriptors: Memory, Gender Differences, Spatial Ability, Recognition (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Pierroutsakos, Sophia L.; DeLoache, Judy S.; Gound, Mary; Bernard, E. Nicole – Developmental Science, 2005
In two experiments on very young children's response to the orientation of pictures and objects, 18-, 24- and 30-month-old children showed no preference for upright pictures over inverted ones. More importantly, we found that children in all three age groups were equally accurate and equally fast at identifying depicted objects regardless of…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Pictorial Stimuli, Task Analysis, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mash, Clay; Novak, Elizabeth; Berthier, Neil E.; Keen, Rachel – Developmental Psychology, 2006
Preferential-looking studies suggest that by 2 months of age, infants may have knowledge about some object properties, such as solidity. Manual search studies of toddlers examining these same concepts, however, have failed to provide evidence for the same understanding. Investigators have recently attempted to reconcile this disparity but failed…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Developmental Psychology, Reaction Time, Psychological Evaluation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Fischer, Kurt W.; Watson, Malcolm W. – Child Development, 1977
A hypothesized developmental sequence of agent use in pretending was tested in 36 infants between 14 and 24 months of age and was compared with the development of object permanence. (JMB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Developmental Stages, Infants, Modeling (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Baillargeon, Renee – Developmental Psychology, 1987
Three experiments test object permanenece in 3 1/2- and 4 1/2-month-old infants, and use an impossible-possible-habituation event format. The 4 1/2-month-olds, and the 3 1/2-month-olds who were fast habituators, look reliably longer at the impossible than at the possible event. Results seriously question Piaget's (1954) claims regarding the age at…
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Habituation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Gopnik, Alison; Meltzoff, Andrew – Child Development, 1987
Changes in children's categorization behavior between 15 and 21 months of age and the relation of these changes to developments in language, object permanence, and means-end understanding are reported. (PCB)
Descriptors: Behavior Change, Classification, Cognitive Development, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Biederman, Irving – Psychological Review, 1987
The theory proposed (recognition-by-components) hypothesizes the perceptual recognition of objects to be a process in which the image of the input is segmented at regions of deep concavity into an arrangement of simple geometric components. Experiments on the perception of briefly presented pictures support the theory. (Author/LMO)
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Pattern Recognition, Psychological Studies, Symmetry
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Black, Kathryn Norcross; Roberts, Gail C. – Child Development, 1972
Purpose of the study was to determine whether a name for a toy affects an infant's attentional preference for a toy and whether this process is related to the infant's concept of subject permanence. (Authors)
Descriptors: Association (Psychology), Attention, Child Language, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
And Others; Rader, Nancy – Child Development, 1979
Examined the role of perceptual-motor development in a typical Stage IV task. The performance of ten infants was compared on a Stage IV object permanence task when a cloth cover was used and when a small card cover was used. (JMB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Infants, Motor Development, Object Permanence
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  7  |  8  |  9  |  10  |  11  |  12  |  13  |  14  |  15  |  ...  |  19