Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 7 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 14 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 24 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 124 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Baillargeon, Renee | 10 |
Johnson, Scott P. | 6 |
Bertenthal, Bennett I. | 5 |
Feigenson, Lisa | 5 |
Call, Josep | 4 |
Carey, Susan | 4 |
Gopnik, Alison | 4 |
Harris, Paul L. | 4 |
Huttenlocher, Janellen | 4 |
Keen, Rachel | 4 |
Luo, Yuyan | 4 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 26 |
Elementary Education | 7 |
Preschool Education | 7 |
Higher Education | 2 |
Primary Education | 2 |
Adult Education | 1 |
High Schools | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 11 |
Practitioners | 10 |
Teachers | 5 |
Parents | 2 |
Location
Australia | 3 |
Belgium | 1 |
Canada | 1 |
Canada (Montreal) | 1 |
Delaware | 1 |
France | 1 |
Germany | 1 |
Guatemala | 1 |
Italy | 1 |
Japan | 1 |
Louisiana | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Bayley Scales of Infant… | 2 |
Bayley Mental Development… | 1 |
Stanford Binet Intelligence… | 1 |
Stroop Color Word Test | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Saxe, Rebecca; Tzelnic, Tania; Carey, Susan – Cognition, 2006
Infants know that humans are exempt from some of the principles that govern the motion of inanimate objects: for instance, humans can be caused to move without being struck. In the current study, we report that infants nevertheless do apply some of the same principles to both humans and objects, where appropriate. Five-month-old infants expect…
Descriptors: Infants, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Ability, Object Permanence
Uttal, David H.; Sandstrom, Lisa B.; Newcombe, Nora S. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2006
An important characteristic of mature spatial cognition is the ability to encode spatial locations in terms of relations among landmarks as well as in terms of vectors that include distance and direction. In this study, we examined children's use of the relation "middle" to code the location of a hidden toy, using a procedure adapted…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Young Children, Toys, Spatial Ability

Gopnik, Alison; Meltzoff, Andrew N. – Child Development, 1992
Eighteen-month-old children performed sorting tasks and their parents completed checklists of words used by the children. Children who performed exhaustive grouping, or grouping of objects of different kinds in different locations, were reported as using more words than children who did not perform exhaustive grouping. (BC)
Descriptors: Classification, Infants, Language Acquisition, Object Permanence

Mitchell, Peter; Taylor, Laura M. – Cognition, 1999
In three shape-constancy experiments, 4- and 7-year olds viewed a circular disc oriented at a slant. All subjects exaggerated circularity of the disc when they knew the object was a circle. Findings suggest that knowledge of reality contaminates judgments of appearance in circle task and this is the same bias that features in realist errors in…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Infants, Learning, Object Permanence
Subbotsky, Eugene – Developmental Psychology, 2005
This study tested participants' preparedness to acknowledge that an object could change as a result of magical intervention. Six- and 9-year-old children and adults treated perceived and imagined objects as being equally permanent. Adults treated a fantastic object as significantly less permanent than either perceived or imagined objects. Results…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Children, Adults, Imagination
Rosander, Kerstin; von Hofsten, Claes – Cognition, 2004
The emerging ability to represent an oscillating moving object over occlusions was studied in 7-21-week-old infants. The object moved at 0.25 Hz and was either occluded at the center of the trajectory (for 0.3 s) or at one turning point (for 0.7 s). Each trial lasted for 20 s. Both eye and head movements were measured. By using two kinds of…
Descriptors: Infants, Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Processes, Object Permanence
Bertenthal, Bennett I.; Longo, Matthew R.; Kenny, Sarah – Child Development, 2007
The perceived spatiotemporal continuity of objects depends on the way they appear and disappear as they move in the spatial layout. This study investigated whether infants' predictive tracking of a briefly occluded object is sensitive to the manner by which the object disappears and reappears. Five-, 7-, and 9-month-old infants were shown a ball…
Descriptors: Kinetics, Infants, Visual Perception, Object Permanence

Sinclair, Hermina – Human Development, 1978
Attempts to clarify the meaning of differentiation in the Piagetian concept of object permanence. (BD)
Descriptors: Conservation (Concept), Developmental Stages, Infant Behavior, Object Permanence

Butterworth, George – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1977
This study tested Piaget's explanation of infants' perseverative error in manual search by comparing searches when the object was (1) hidden, (2) covered but visible, and (3) uncovered. Errors occurred under all three conditions, with conflict at a maximum when the object was hidden. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Egocentrism, Error Patterns, Infants

Wellman, Henry M.; And Others – Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 1967
Statistical analysis of many studies on Piaget's stage 4 object concept attempts to synthesize available quantitative data. Factors of this "meta-analysis" include: (1) effects of age; (2) number of A trials; (3) length of delay between hiding and search; (4) number of locations; and (5) visual properties of hiding arrays. Includes…
Descriptors: Infants, Meta Analysis, Object Permanence, Piagetian Theory

Lingle, Kathleen M.; Lingle, John H. – Child Development, 1981
Investigates the degree to which object familiarity and motivational factors influence infants' search behavior in an object-permanence test. (Author/MP)
Descriptors: Infant Behavior, Infants, Measures (Individuals), Motivation

Luo, Yuyan; Baillargeon, Renee; Brueckner, Laura; Munakata, Yuko – Cognition, 2003
This study examined two alternative interpretations of violation-of-expectation findings that young infants can represent hidden objects. Findings indicated that 5-month-olds succeeded in reasoning about the interaction of a visible and a hidden object even though the 2 objects were never simultaneously visible and a 3- or 4-minute delay preceded…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Infant Behavior, Infants, Memory

Kellman, Philip J.; Shipley, Thomas F. – Cognitive Psychology, 1991
A theory is presented to explain the perception of partially occluded objects and illusory figures, from both static and kinematic information, in a unified framework. This detailed theory of unit formation accounts for most cases of boundary perception in the absence of local physical specification. (SLD)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Concept Formation, Object Permanence, Theories
Wang, S.h.; Baillargeon, R.; Paterson, S. – Cognition, 2005
Recent research on infants' responses to occlusion and containment events indicates that, although some violations of the continuity principle are detected at an early age e.g. Aguiar, A., & Baillargeon, R. (1999). 2.5-month-old infants' reasoning about when objects should and should not be occluded. Cognitive Psychology 39, 116-157; Hespos, S.…
Descriptors: Infant Behavior, Object Permanence, Cognitive Psychology, Child Development
Dimitrova, Daniela V.; Bugeja, Michael – portal: Libraries and the Academy, 2006
This study focuses on six leading communication journals and their use of online citations in articles published between 2000 and 2003. The study uses content analysis to explore if there is a relationship between citation characteristics and their stability. The findings show that online citations in the .gov and .org domains are more likely to…
Descriptors: Periodicals, Citations (References), Internet, Online Searching