Publication Date
In 2025 | 2 |
Since 2024 | 11 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 40 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 80 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 239 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Levin, Joel R. | 17 |
Smeets, Paul M. | 11 |
Ghatala, Elizabeth S. | 9 |
Klausmeier, Herbert J. | 9 |
Spiker, Charles C. | 9 |
Cantor, Joan H. | 8 |
Gholson, Barry | 7 |
Goulet, L. R. | 7 |
Reed, Phil | 7 |
Schreibman, Laura | 7 |
Siegel, Alexander W. | 7 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Researchers | 70 |
Practitioners | 23 |
Teachers | 7 |
Location
Australia | 8 |
Canada | 7 |
Ohio | 5 |
Spain | 5 |
United Kingdom (England) | 4 |
Brazil | 2 |
France | 2 |
Massachusetts | 2 |
Mexico | 2 |
Netherlands | 2 |
New York | 2 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 1 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 1 |
Ramscar, Michael – Journal of Child Language, 2021
How do children learn to communicate, and what do they learn? Traditionally, most theories have taken an associative, compositional approach to these questions, supposing children acquire an inventory of form-meaning associations, and procedures for composing / decomposing them; into / from messages in production and comprehension. This paper…
Descriptors: Child Language, Communication Skills, Discrimination Learning, Learning Theories
Fuhs, Mary Wagner; Tavassolie, Nadia; Wang, Yiqiao; Bartek, Victoria; Sheeks, Natalie A.; Gunderson, Elizabeth A. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2021
Young children are sensitive to both numerical and spatial magnitude cues early in development, but many questions remain about how children's attention to magnitudes relates to their early math achievement. In two studies, we tested three hypotheses related to the flexible attention to magnitudes (FAM) account, which suggests that young…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Mathematics Skills, Numeracy, Number Concepts
Don, Hilary J.; Goldwater, Micah B.; Greenaway, Justine K.; Hutchings, Rosalind; Livesey, Evan J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2020
Failure to learn and generalize abstract relational rules has critical implications for education. In this study, we aimed to determine which training conditions facilitate relational transfer in a relatively simple (patterning) discrimination versus a relatively complex (biconditional) discrimination. The amount of training participants received…
Descriptors: Discrimination Learning, Transfer of Training, Cognitive Processes, Reflection
Bowman, Caitlin R.; Zeithamova, Dagmar – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2020
Building conceptual knowledge that generalizes to novel situations is a key function of human memory. Category-learning paradigms have long been used to understand the mechanisms of knowledge generalization. In the present study, we tested the conditions that promote formation of new concepts. Participants underwent 1 of 6 training conditions that…
Descriptors: Concept Formation, Generalization, Discrimination Learning, Classification
Yunji Park; Alexandria A. Viegut; Percival G. Matthews – Grantee Submission, 2021
Humans perceptually extract quantity information from our environments, be it from simple stimuli in isolation, or from relational magnitudes formed by taking ratios of pairs of simple stimuli. Some have proposed that these two types of magnitude are processed by a common system, whereas others have proposed separate systems. To test these…
Descriptors: Mathematics Skills, Thinking Skills, Preschool Children, Elementary School Students
Akers, Jessica S.; Retzlaff, Billie J.; Fisher, Wayne W.; Greer, Brian D.; Kaminski, Ami J.; DeSouza, Andresa A. – Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 2019
Most verbal behavior curricula for children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) focus on teaching multiple mands during the early stages of training (e.g., picture exchange communication system; Bondy & Frost, 1994). However, few, if any, of those curricula train children with ASD to differentially mand only for reinforcers that are reasonable…
Descriptors: Behavior Modification, Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Training
Ferry, Alissa; Nespor, Marina; Mehler, Jacques – Developmental Psychology, 2020
To learn a language infants must learn to link arbitrary sounds to their meaning. While words are the clearest example of this link, they are not the only component of language; morphological regularities (e.g., the plural -s suffix in English) carry meaning as well. Comprehensive theories of language acquisition must account for how infants build…
Descriptors: Infants, Child Language, Comprehension, Morphology (Languages)
Rose, Jane; Flaherty, Mary; Browning, Jenna; Leibold, Lori J.; Buss, Emily – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2018
Purpose: Published data indicate nearly adultlike frequency discrimination in infants but large child -adult differences for school-age children. This study evaluated the role that differences in measurement procedures and stimuli may have played in the apparent nonmonotonicity. Frequency discrimination was assessed in preschoolers, young…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Young Children, Adults, Auditory Discrimination
Kalashnikova, Marina; Goswami, Usha; Burnham, Denis – Developmental Science, 2019
Here we report, for the first time, a relationship between sensitivity to amplitude envelope rise time in infants and their later vocabulary development. Recent research in auditory neuroscience has revealed that amplitude envelope rise time plays a mechanistic role in speech encoding. Accordingly, individual differences in infant discrimination…
Descriptors: Infants, Auditory Perception, Vocabulary Development, Speech
Yan, Veronica X.; Sana, Faria – Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 2019
Interleaving examples of to-be-learned categories, rather than blocking examples by category, frequently enhances category induction. The presently dominant theory is that interleaving promotes discriminative-contrast, and suggests that category similarity structure modulates this interleaving benefit: that blocking should benefit learning when…
Descriptors: Learning Processes, Learning Strategies, Discrimination Learning, Learning Theories
Foilb, Allison R.; Bals, Julia; Sarlitto, Mary C.; Christianson, John P. – Learning & Memory, 2018
Distinguishing safety from danger is necessary for survival, but is aberrant in individuals with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). While PTSD is more prevalent in women than men, research on sex differences in safety learning is limited. Here, female rats demonstrated greater fear discrimination than males in a CS+/CS- paradigm. To determine…
Descriptors: Gender Differences, Posttraumatic Stress Disorder, Fear, Discrimination Learning
Moore, James W.; Russo, Kayla; Gilfeather, Angelina; Whipple, Heather M.; Stanford, Greg – Journal of Behavioral Education, 2018
Children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) often emit errors during the establishment of conditional discriminations. These children may not respond to more traditional error-correction procedures, such as least-to-most prompting. In this study, we compared two other types of error-correction procedures, namely embedding an identity-matching…
Descriptors: Error Correction, Children, Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorders
Agarwal, Pooja K.; Agostinelli, Anne – American Educator, 2020
One of the most fundamental strategies in mathematics instruction is practice problems because teachers know that practicing a skill improves performance. At the same time, teachers also know that just because students can correctly answer practice problems does not mean they fully understand the concept or how to apply a formula--especially not…
Descriptors: Mathematics Instruction, Teaching Methods, Concept Teaching, Mathematical Concepts
Kandalam, Suraj; Ferro, Tyler; Pawluk, Dianne T. V. – Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 2019
Introduction: This article describes the development and initial assessment of a set of tactile crayons that can produce different textured lines and areas on standard paper. Method: An assessment of the need for tactile crayons was performed through the use of surveys and focus groups from which a set of six tactile crayons was developed.…
Descriptors: Freehand Drawing, Art Materials, Manipulative Materials, Visual Impairments
Rollins, Leslie; Cloude, Elizabeth B. – Learning & Memory, 2018
The present study examined mnemonic discrimination in 5- and 6-yr-old children, 8- and 9-yr-old children, 11- and 12-yr-old children, and young adults. Participants incidentally encoded pictorial stimuli and subsequently judged whether targets (i.e., repeated stimuli), lures (i.e., mnemonically related stimuli), and foils (i.e., novel stimuli)…
Descriptors: Mnemonics, Discrimination Learning, Children, Preadolescents