Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 5 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 9 |
Descriptor
Associative Learning | 26 |
Vocabulary Development | 26 |
Word Recognition | 26 |
Teaching Methods | 8 |
Language Acquisition | 7 |
Second Language Instruction | 5 |
Foreign Countries | 4 |
Language Processing | 4 |
Phonology | 4 |
Reading Instruction | 4 |
Second Language Learning | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 19 |
Reports - Research | 13 |
Guides - Classroom - Teacher | 4 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 3 |
Opinion Papers | 2 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 2 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 2 | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Kindergarten | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Audience
Practitioners | 2 |
Teachers | 2 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
MacArthur Bates Communicative… | 1 |
Wechsler Preschool and… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Guerra, Ernesto; Kronmüller, Edmundo – Reading Research Quarterly, 2020
An important component of reading comprehension is the reader's capacity to make inferences that can maintain the coherence between propositions within the text. However, the cognitive and linguistic skills that underlie online inference making remain elusive. The authors aimed to clarify the effects of vocabulary and text comprehension on…
Descriptors: Reading Comprehension, Inferences, Vocabulary, Word Recognition
Tsui, Angeline Sin Mei; Byers-Heinlein, Krista; Fennell, Christopher T. – Developmental Psychology, 2019
Associative word learning, the ability to pair a concept to a word, is an essential mechanism for early language development. One common method by which researchers measure this ability is the Switch task (Werker, Cohen, Lloyd, Casasola, & Stager, 1998), wherein infants are habituated to 2 word-object pairings and then tested on their ability…
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Vocabulary Development, Language Acquisition, Infants
Feng, Ye; Kager, René; Lai, Regine; Wong, Patrick C. M. – Developmental Psychology, 2022
The ability to map similar sounding words to different meanings alone is far from enough for successful speech processing. To overcome variability in the speech signal, young learners must also recognize words across surface variations. Previous studies have shown that infants at 14 months are able to use variations in word-internal cues (i.e.,…
Descriptors: Infants, Developmental Stages, Phonology, Intonation
Kuhlmann, Beatrice G.; Brubaker, Matthew S.; Pfeiffer, Theresa; Naveh-Benjamin, Moshe – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2021
Few studies have compared interference-based forgetting between item versus associative memory. The memory-system dependent forgetting hypothesis (Hardt, Nader, & Nadel, 2013) predicts that effects of interference on associative memory should be minimal because its hippocampal representation allows pattern separation even of highly similar…
Descriptors: Older Adults, Memory, Comparative Analysis, Interference (Learning)
Valente, Daniela; Ferré, Pilar; Soares, Ana; Rato, Anabela; Comesaña, Montserrat – Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics, 2018
Very few studies exist on the role of cross-language similarities in cognate word acquisition. Here we sought to explore, for the first time, the interplay of orthography (O) and phonology (P) during the early stages of cognate word acquisition, looking at children and adults with the same level of foreign language proficiency and by using two…
Descriptors: Phonology, Language Processing, Second Language Learning, Native Language
McMurray, Bob; Horst, Jessica S.; Samuelson, Larissa K. – Psychological Review, 2012
Classic approaches to word learning emphasize referential ambiguity: In naming situations, a novel word could refer to many possible objects, properties, actions, and so forth. To solve this, researchers have posited constraints, and inference strategies, but assume that determining the referent of a novel word is isomorphic to learning. We…
Descriptors: Word Recognition, Vocabulary Development, Language Acquisition, Interaction
Boudewyn, Megan A.; Gordon, Peter C.; Long, Debra; Polse, Lara; Swaab, Tamara Y. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2012
The goal of this study was to examine how lexical association and discourse congruence affect the time course of processing incoming words in spoken discourse. In an event-related potential (ERP) norming study, we presented prime-target pairs in the absence of a sentence context to obtain a baseline measure of lexical priming. We observed a…
Descriptors: Priming, Evidence, Comprehension, Sentences
Curtin, Suzanne – Journal of Child Language, 2009
Infants at 1;2 demonstrate difficulty in accessing subtle phonetic information about newly learned word-object pairings (Stager & Werker, 1997). In this study, we examined whether or not infants can access subtle prosodic information such as lexical stress in a word learning task. We tested infants younger than 1;2 to see if they could learn two…
Descriptors: Suprasegmentals, Infants, Associative Learning, Word Recognition
Byrne, Brian; Wadsworth, Sally; Boehme, Kristi; Talk, Andrew C.; Coventry, William L.; Olson, Richard K.; Samuelsson, Stefan; Corley, Robin – Scientific Studies of Reading, 2013
The genetic factor structure of a range of learning measures was explored in twin children, recruited in preschool and followed to Grade 2 ("N"?=?2,084). Measures of orthographic learning and word reading were included in the analyses to determine how these patterned with the learning processes. An exploratory factor analysis of the…
Descriptors: Genetics, Preschool Children, Elementary School Students, Kindergarten

Mervis, Carolyn B. – Child Development, 1994
Three studies examined two-year olds' understanding of novel terms for objects that they are already familiar with under another name. The studies found that the new term was most likely to be treated as a second basic-level name for the category to which the object belonged. (MDM)
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Classification, Toddlers, Vocabulary Development
Blasingame, James, Jr.; Nilsen, Alleen Pace – English Journal, 2005
A lesson focusing on the names of muscles but relating them to more common words is presented, as current research suggests that the best way to teach vocabulary is to group related words. Students create visual representations of word groups and teach the words to the class.
Descriptors: Vocabulary Development, Teaching Methods, Visual Learning, Associative Learning

Mueller, Donald J.; Gumina, James M. – Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1972
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Cognitive Processes, Learning Processes, Learning Theories
FLEMING, GERALD – 1966
THE AUTHOR'S POSITION WAS THAT, BECAUSE PEOPLE OFTEN DIFFER IN THEIR UNDERSTANDING OF THE MEANING OF WORDS, LINGUISTS MUST WORK WITH PSYCHOLOGISTS, NEUROLOGISTS, AND OTHERS TO GUIDE LANGUAGE TEACHERS IN THEIR KNOWLEDGE OF HOW A LANGUAGE WORKS AND HOW BEST TO COMMUNICATE THIS KNOWLEDGE TO THEIR STUDENTS. WHILE DENOTATIVE MEANING REFERS PRIMARILY TO…
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Behavior Patterns, Communication (Thought Transfer), Comprehension
Dale, Edgar – 1972
In our concern to improve the quality of reading, we have neglected vocabulary development; in our zeal to teach children to pronounce words, we have neglected teaching them word meanings. Vocabulary can be developed by using all the communication processes: reading, writing, speaking, listening, visualizing, and observing; and vocabulary deserves…
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Concept Formation, Early Experience, Environmental Influences
Milligan, Jerry L.; O'Toole, Raymond J. – Elem Sch J, 1969
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Elementary School Science, Linguistic Theory, Science Instruction
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2