Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 4 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Development | 6 |
Object Permanence | 6 |
Spatial Ability | 6 |
Infants | 4 |
Age Differences | 3 |
Task Analysis | 3 |
Attention | 2 |
Developmental Stages | 2 |
Psychomotor Skills | 2 |
Age | 1 |
Animals | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Developmental Science | 2 |
American Journal of Mental… | 1 |
Cognitive Development | 1 |
Infancy | 1 |
Journal of Experimental Child… | 1 |
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Research | 6 |
Numerical/Quantitative Data | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
O'Hearn, Kirsten; Hoffman, James E.; Landau, Barbara – Developmental Science, 2010
The ability to track moving objects, a crucial skill for mature performance on everyday spatial tasks, has been hypothesized to require a specialized mechanism that may be available in infancy (i.e. indexes). Consistent with the idea of specialization, our previous work showed that object tracking was more impaired than a matched spatial memory…
Descriptors: Genetic Disorders, Object Permanence, Age, Infants
Perry, Lynn K.; Samuelson, Larissa K.; Spencer, John P. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2009
This study investigated how young children's increasingly flexible use of spatial reference frames enables accurate search for hidden objects by using a task that 3-year-olds have been shown to perform with great accuracy and 2-year-olds have been shown to perform inaccurately. Children watched as an object was rolled down a ramp, behind a panel…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Preschool Children, Task Analysis, Experiments
Feigenson, Lisa; Yamaguchi, Mariko – Infancy, 2009
Like adults, infants use working memory to represent occluded objects and can update these memory representations to reflect changes to a scene that unfold over time. Here we tested the limits of infants' ability to update object representations in working memory. Eleven-month-old infants participated in a modified foraging task in which they saw…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Infants, Short Term Memory, Cognitive Processes
Okamoto-Barth, Sanae; Tomonaga, Masaki; Tanaka, Masayuki; Matsuzawa, Tetsuro – Developmental Science, 2008
The use of gaze shifts as social cues has various evolutionary advantages. To investigate the developmental processes of this ability, we conducted an object-choice task by using longitudinal methods with infant chimpanzees tested from 8 months old until 3 years old. The experimenter used one of six gestures towards a cup concealing food; tapping,…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Cues, Behavioral Science Research, Infants

Silverstein, A. B.; And Others – American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1982
Scales for object permanence and spatial relationships were administered to 98 severely and profoundly mentally retarded children (mean age 13 years) on three occasions, 6 months apart. Differences in the difficulty of the items were quite stable, but their order of difficulty differed appreciably from that for nonretarded infants. (Author/SB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Developmental Stages, Object Permanence, Severe Mental Retardation

Rochat, Philippe; Hespos, Susan J. – Cognitive Development, 1996
Examines the ability of infants to track and anticipate the final orientation of an object. Subjects were infants ranging from an average of four months to eight months old. Three experiments, with the last one as control, were carried out. Concludes that infants show some rudimentary mental rotation from four months of age. (MOK)
Descriptors: Basic Skills, Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Development, Infants