NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Publication Date
In 20250
Since 20240
Since 2021 (last 5 years)0
Since 2016 (last 10 years)1
Since 2006 (last 20 years)6
Education Level
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Mean Length of Utterance1
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 50 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lobo, Maria; Santos, Ana Lúcia; Soares-Jesel, Carla – Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics, 2016
This article investigates the acquisition of different types of clefts and of "be"-fragments in European Portuguese. We first present the main syntactic and discourse properties of different cleft structures and of "be"-fragments in European Portuguese, and we discuss how data from first language acquisition may contribute to…
Descriptors: Portuguese, Language Acquisition, Task Analysis, Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Theakston, Anna L. – Applied Psycholinguistics, 2012
In this study, 5-year-olds and adults described scenes that differed according to whether (a) the subject or object of a transitive verb represented an accessible or inaccessible referent, consistent or inconsistent with patterns of preferred argument structure, and (b) a simple noun was sufficient to uniquely identify an inaccessible referent.…
Descriptors: Language Patterns, Sentences, Nouns, Adults
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Salomo, Dorothe; Graf, Eileen; Lieven, Elena; Tomasello, Michael – Journal of Child Language, 2011
Three- and four-year-old children were asked predicate-focus questions ("What's X doing?") about a scene in which an agent performed an action on a patient. We varied: (i) whether (or not) the preceding discourse context, which established the patient as given information, was available for the questioner; and (ii) whether (or not) the patient was…
Descriptors: Context Effect, Young Children, Role, Questioning Techniques
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Zukowski, Andrea; Larsen, Jaiva – Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics, 2011
Previous research has suggested that very young children learning English adhere quite rigidly to a grammatical constraint on the possible contexts for contraction of "want" and "to" into the reduced form "wanna". Two elicited production studies reported here suggest that young children do produce "wanna" in illicit contexts. One study identifies…
Descriptors: Young Children, Language Processing, English (Second Language), Second Language Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Shimpi, Priya M.; Fedewa, Alicia; Hans, Sydney – Applied Psycholinguistics, 2012
The relation of social and linguistic input measures to early vocabulary development was examined in 30 low-income African American mother-infant pairs. Observations were conducted when the child was 0 years, 1 month (0;1), 0;4, 0;8, 1;0, 1;6, and 2;0. Maternal input was coded for word types and tokens, contingent responsiveness, and…
Descriptors: Outcome Measures, Correlation, Longitudinal Studies, Child Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Uccelli, Paola – Journal of Child Language, 2009
This study describes how young Spanish-speaking children become gradually more adept at encoding temporality using grammar and discourse skills in intra-conversational narratives. The research involved parallel case studies of two Spanish-speaking children followed longitudinally from ages two to three. Type/token frequencies of verb tense,…
Descriptors: Speech Communication, Verbs, Morphemes, Discourse Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Bohannon, John Neil, III; Leubecker, Amye Warren – Language Sciences, 1988
Describes a model that allows children to control the complexity of the speech they hear within conversations on a moment-to-moment basis. Experimental and observational data clearly delineate the reciprocal nature of how speakers "fine-tune" their speech to listeners. The effects of child-directed speech on language development are discussed.…
Descriptors: Child Language, Discourse Analysis, Language Patterns, Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
McCabe, Allyssa; Peterson, Carole – Journal of Child Language, 1985
Describes a study that analyzes the naturalistic productions of "because" and "so" by 96 children, aged three-and-a-half to nine-and-a-half years of age, while narrating personal events. Analyzes results in terms of such factors as: correctness, types of causality, nature of actor/recipient, time of causality, producer, and linguistic issues. (SED)
Descriptors: Child Language, Concept Formation, Connected Discourse, Discourse Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Tfouni, Leda Verdiani; Klatzky, Robert L. – Journal of Child Language, 1983
Findings include (1) comprehension of 'this,''that,''here,' and 'there' depends on the role the comprehender plays in the conversation and (2) 'this' and 'here' are more difficult to comprehend that 'that' and 'there.' (EKN)
Descriptors: Child Language, Discourse Analysis, Language Acquisition, Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Cacciari, Cristina; Levorato, Maria Chiara – Journal of Child Language, 1989
Three experiments gauging 7- to 10-year-old children's ability to interpret and comprehend figurative language indicated that informative contexts could improve subjects' abilities to perceive idiomatic meanings. Subjects were less able to produce idioms than to comprehend them, but were able to perceive that language can be both figurative and…
Descriptors: Child Language, Discourse Analysis, Figurative Language, Idioms
Wootten, Janet; And Others – 1979
The use of "wh" forms in questions asked by four children was recorded from age 22 to 36 months, and analyzed. In the emergence of "wh" forms, the children first asked identifying questions with "what" and "who," followed in order by (1) "wh" pronominal questions which ask for major sentence…
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Development, Discourse Analysis, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Weist, Richard W.; And Others – Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1982
Children listened to stories which contained anomalies produced by violating semantic restrictions or based on conflicting propositions at two points in a story. The capacity to detect violations of sentences developed more rapidly than detection of violation of discourse. Children's developing capacity to integrate and store story structure is…
Descriptors: Child Language, Connected Discourse, Discourse Analysis, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Sealey, Alison – Current Issues in Language and Society, 1996
Explores how children negotiated social identities through language used in spontaneous, casual conversation. Examines how audio transcripts of the childrens' conversations exhibit different aspects of their social "selves" and suggests that their talk can be analyzed in relation to the individual self, the immediately situated social actor, and…
Descriptors: Audiotape Recordings, Child Language, Context Effect, Discourse Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Baker, Nancy D.; Greenfield, Patricia M. – Language Sciences, 1988
A longitudinal study of four 17- to 33-month-olds revealed that their linguistic selection at the one-word stage was governed by principles of informativeness, while the two-word stage was characterized by new, or a combination of new and old, information. (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Child Language, Discourse Analysis, Language Acquisition, Language Patterns
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Horgan, Dianne – Journal of Child Language, 1978
How a child answers questions provides information about how he or she processes input. A child's early responses to questions at age one year, three months, were compared to her responses at one year, seven months, when she was in the two-word stage. (SW)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Comprehension, Discourse Analysis
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4