Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 1 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 49 |
Language Acquisition | 49 |
Verbal Development | 49 |
Child Language | 30 |
Psycholinguistics | 28 |
Cognitive Development | 23 |
Language Research | 22 |
Preschool Children | 16 |
Language Processing | 12 |
Longitudinal Studies | 11 |
Syntax | 11 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Bloom, Lois | 2 |
Farwell, Carol B. | 2 |
Horgan, Dianne | 2 |
Akiyama, M. Michael | 1 |
Anna Sofia Bratt | 1 |
Antinucci, Francesco | 1 |
Ausubel, David P. | 1 |
Baron, Naomi S. | 1 |
Bartelo, Dennise M. | 1 |
Beiswenger, Hugo | 1 |
Ben-Zeev, Sandra | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Research | 26 |
Journal Articles | 9 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 3 |
Dissertations/Theses | 1 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
Sweden | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Home Observation for… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Emma Bergström; Anna Sofia Bratt; Idor Svensson – Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 2024
Creating an environment suitable for language acquisition through shared reading significantly contributes to improving a child's language development and parent-child relationship. Reading in an interactive way, such as dialogic reading, is favorable. Nevertheless, dialogic reading is designed for children above the age of two and shared reading…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Infants, Language Acquisition, Early Reading

Skarakis, Elizabeth; Greenfield, Patricia M. – Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1982
Results showed that 12 language disordered children (four to six years old) selectively marked new information in verbal communication, just as normal children do. Language disordered and normal children, furthermore, manifested the same developmental sequence of strategies for deemphasizing old information. (Author/CL)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Elementary Education, Language Acquisition, Language Handicaps

Akiyama, M. Michael; Wilcox, Sharon A. – Journal of Child Language, 1993
Experiments with groups of 30 children (aged 3 through 6) and 32 children (aged 5 through 8) showed that (1) children use linguistic form-class information with familiar discrete objects, (2) children do not use linguistic form-class information with familiar food, and (3) children use only object category information with unfamiliar items.…
Descriptors: Classification, Cognitive Processes, Form Classes (Languages), Language Acquisition

Moerk, Ernst L. – 1973
The following are sketched in outline form: (1) functional antecedents and their implications for language--assimilation, accommodation, circular reactions/ feedback processes, classification, discrimination, functional equivalence, representation, transformation, communications; (2) semantic antecedents and their implications for language--human…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Infant Behavior, Infants

Horgan, Dianne – Journal of Child Language, 1978
Spontaneous full passives and related constructions from 234 children, aged 2 to 13, and elicited passives from 262 college students were analyzed. The agentive non-reversible did not appear until after age 9; and until age 11 no child produced both reversible and non-reversible passives. (Author/SW)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Language Acquisition, Language Research

Leonard, Laurence B.; Schwartz, Richard G. – Journal of Child Language, 1978
Focus is one factor that may account for children's use of single-word utterances after they have acquired the use of multi-word utterances. The possible role that focus may play in children's use of single-word utterances in naturalistic settings, after the acquisition of syntax, was investigated. (SW)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Language Acquisition, Language Research

Ninio, Anat – Journal of Child Language, 1980
Ostensive definitions of words are ambiguities as to their referent. In a study of infant-mother dyads engaged in looking at picture books, 95 percent of ostensive definitions referred to the whole object depicted rather than parts, attributes, or actions. When parts were named, ambiguity was avoided by naming the part and the whole. (PJM)
Descriptors: Ambiguity, Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Language Acquisition
Gever, Benson E.; Weisberg, Robert W. – 1970
This investigation gives attention to the developmental course and influence of social class on self-directed, private speech. The Ss were 108 white, middle and lower class pre-school, first and third grade children. Two measures of receptive vocabulary were administered as background language measures. The experimental task required S to sort…
Descriptors: Ability, Age, Children, Cognitive Processes

Peters, Ann M. – Language, 1977
Reports on a child who evidently used a gestalt strategy (proceeding from the whole to the parts) in learning his first language. Further evidence for a gestalt strategy exists in the literature, albeit implicitly, and any theory of language or language acquisition should be able to account for it. (Author/KM)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Language Acquisition, Language Learning Levels

Bloom, Lois; And Others – Cognitive Psychology, 1976
The discourse interaction between adult and child was examined in terms of the content of their utterances, and the linguistic and contextual relations between their messages, in order to investigate how children use the information from adults' input sentences to form contingent responses. (Author/BW)
Descriptors: Adults, Cognitive Processes, Discourse Analysis, Interaction

Kuczaj, Stan A., II – Journal of Child Language, 1978
The progressive inflection "-ing" appears to be the earliest verb inflection acquired by children learning English as their first language. Explanations are made on why the progressive is rarely, if ever, overgeneralized to inappropriate forms. (SW)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Concept Formation, Generalization

Antinucci, Francesco; Miller, Ruth – Journal of Child Language, 1976
Investigates the development of past tense expressions in the speech of children from 1.6 to 2.6. It is shown that this development depends crucially on the child's cognitive construction of the time dimension, as described by Piaget. (Author/RM)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Language Acquisition
Ausubel, David P. – Claremont Coll Reading Conf 33rd Yearbook, 1969
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Experience, Language Acquisition
Ghatala, Elizabeth S.; And Others – 1975
This study incorporated a correlational methodology into an experimental context to determine the functional components of rehearsal strategies in children's discrimination learning. The subjects for this study were 120 fifth- and sixth-grade children attending two elementary schools located in middle-class areas of Ogden, Utah. According to the…
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Discrimination Learning, Educational Research

Vogel, Irene – 1975
Many researchers have assumed that adult bilinguals have separate systems for their two languages. Such an assumption raises interesting questions about how the two languages are acquired in the case of a child learning two languages simultaneously. This study attempts to determine whether the two languages are acquired separately right from the…
Descriptors: Bilingualism, Child Language, Cognitive Processes, English