Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 1 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 8 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 21 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 29 |
Descriptor
Source
Developmental Psychology | 51 |
Author
Harris, Paul L. | 5 |
Friedman, Ori | 3 |
Corriveau, Kathleen H. | 2 |
Keil, Frank C. | 2 |
Killen, Melanie | 2 |
Abadie, Marlène | 1 |
Abrams, Dominic | 1 |
Alibali, Martha W. | 1 |
Anderson, Jacob E. | 1 |
Andrew Shtulman | 1 |
Aneja, Alka | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 48 |
Reports - Research | 46 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 1 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 6 |
Early Childhood Education | 4 |
Higher Education | 2 |
Postsecondary Education | 2 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 1 |
Grade 2 | 1 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
Intermediate Grades | 1 |
Preschool Education | 1 |
Primary Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 7 |
Location
Canada | 7 |
China | 3 |
Germany | 2 |
Massachusetts | 2 |
Australia | 1 |
California (Los Angeles) | 1 |
Connecticut | 1 |
France | 1 |
Iowa | 1 |
Michigan | 1 |
Minnesota | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
NEO Personality Inventory | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Andrew Shtulman; Brandon Goulding; Ori Friedman – Developmental Psychology, 2024
Young children tend to deny the possibility of events that violate their expectations, including events that are merely improbable, like making onion-flavored ice cream or owning a crocodile as a pet. Could this tendency be countered by teaching children more valid strategies for judging possibility? We explored this question by training children…
Descriptors: Children, Thinking Skills, Evaluative Thinking, Age Differences
Sehl, Claudia G.; Denison, Stephanie; Friedman, Ori – Developmental Psychology, 2023
Children have a robust social preference for people similar to them, like those who share their language, accent, and race. In the present research, we show that this preference can diminish when children consider who they want to learn about. Across three experiments, 4- to 6-year-olds (total N = 160; 74 female, 86 male, from the Waterloo region…
Descriptors: Childrens Attitudes, Inferences, Social Cognition, Familiarity
Payir, Ayse; Heiphetz, Larisa; Harris, Paul L.; Corriveau, Kathleen H. – Developmental Psychology, 2022
Recent research has shown that a religious upbringing renders children receptive to ordinarily impossible outcomes, but the underlying mechanism for this effect remains unclear. Exposure to religious teachings might alter children's basic understanding of causality. Alternatively, religious exposure might only affect children's religious…
Descriptors: Children, Religious Factors, Religious Education, Cognitive Development
Parr, Morgan N. Di Napoli; O'Neal, Elizabeth E.; Zhou, Shiwen; Williams, Breanna; Butler, Katherine M.; Chen, Andy; Kearney, Joseph K.; Plumert, Jodie M. – Developmental Psychology, 2023
This investigation examined whether the mode of locomotion matters in how 8-, 10-, 12-, and 14-year-old children (N = 91) judge dynamic affordances in a complex perception-action task with significant safety risks. The primarily European American children in the sample came from the area of Iowa City, Iowa and were balanced for gender. The same…
Descriptors: Children, Adolescents, Physical Activities, Affordances
Waroquier, Laurent; Abadie, Marlène; Blaye, Agnès – Developmental Psychology, 2022
Evaluative conditioning (EC) refers to a change in liking of a conditioned stimulus (CS) consecutive to its repeated pairing with a valent unconditioned stimulus (US). We relied on a multinomial processing tree model to compare the processes underlying EC in middle-aged children (n = 57, M[subscript age] = 8.65, range = 6.94-11.03; 31 females) and…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Young Adults, Evaluative Thinking
Derksen, Daniel G.; Giroux, Megan E.; Newman, Eryn J.; Bernstein, Daniel M. – Developmental Psychology, 2022
When semantically-related photos appear with true-or-false trivia claims, people more often rate the claims as true compared to when photos are absent--"truthiness." This occurs even when the photos lack information useful for assessing veracity. We tested whether truthiness changed in magnitude as a function of participants' age in a…
Descriptors: Credibility, Semantics, Evaluative Thinking, Age Groups
Girouard-Hallam, Lauren N.; Danovitch, Judith H. – Developmental Psychology, 2022
As children increasingly interact with digital voice assistants, it is important to know whether they treat these devices as reliable information sources. Two studies investigated children's trust in and recall of statements made by a novel voice assistant and a human informant. In Study 1, children ages 4-5 (M[subscript age] = 5.05; 20 boys, 20…
Descriptors: Artificial Intelligence, Assistive Technology, Trust (Psychology), Preschool Children
Cracking the Code of Place Value: The Relationship between "Place" and "Value" Takes Years to Master
Cheung, Pierina; Ansari, Daniel – Developmental Psychology, 2021
"Place value," which underlies the meanings of multidigits, encompasses the principle of position and base-10 rules. To understand 65, one needs to know that the digits 6 and 5 occupy different positions and thus represent ordered values of different magnitudes (i.e., the "principle of position") and that the value of each…
Descriptors: Number Concepts, Children, Child Development, Age Differences
Giving a Larger Amount or a Larger Proportion: Stimulus Format Impacts Children's Social Evaluations
Hurst, Michelle A.; Shaw, Alex; Chernyak, Nadia; Levine, Susan C. – Developmental Psychology, 2020
Young children show remarkably sophisticated abilities to evaluate others. Yet their abilities to engage in proportional moral evaluation undergoes protracted development. Namely, young children evaluate someone who shares "absolutely" more as being "nicer" than someone who shares "proportionally" more (e.g., sharing…
Descriptors: Young Children, Adults, Decision Making, Moral Values
Noyes, Alexander; Dunham, Yarrow; Keil, Frank C. – Developmental Psychology, 2020
When faced with entities with potentially ambiguous category membership, adult category judgments are strongly biased toward dangerous and distinctive properties. For example, a cyanide-water mixture is categorized as cyanide. We used a developmental approach to better understand this cross-domain effect, which we term the asymmetric…
Descriptors: Bias, Classification, Evaluative Thinking, Attention
Rizzo, Michael T.; Killen, Melanie – Developmental Psychology, 2020
Social inequalities limit important opportunities and resources for members of marginalized and disadvantaged groups. Understanding the origins of how children construct their understanding of social inequalities in the context of their everyday peer interactions has the potential to yield novel insights into when--and how--individuals respond to…
Descriptors: Status, Justice, Disadvantaged, Children
Cui, Yixin Kelly; Clegg, Jennifer M.; Yan, Eleanor Fang; Davoodi, Telli; Harris, Paul L.; Corriveau, Kathleen H. – Developmental Psychology, 2020
When learning about the existence of unobservable scientific phenomena such as germs or religious phenomena such as God, children are receptive to the testimony of other people. Research in Western cultures has shown that by 5 to 6 years of age, children--like adults--are confident about the existence of both scientific and religious phenomena. We…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Childrens Attitudes, Parent Attitudes, Beliefs
Nancekivell, Shaylene E.; Friedman, Ori – Developmental Psychology, 2017
In two experiments (N = 64), we told 6- to 7-year-olds about improbable or impossible outcomes (Experiment 1) and about impossible outcomes concerning ordinary or magical agents (Experiment 2). In both experiments, children claimed that the outcomes were impossible and could not happen, but nonetheless generated realistic and natural explanations…
Descriptors: Young Children, Logical Thinking, Foreign Countries, Prediction
Huh, Michelle; Friedman, Ori – Developmental Psychology, 2019
In 4 experiments, we show that young children (total N = 290) use information about supply and demand to infer the desirability of resources. In each experiment, children saw scenarios about sandwiches from different shops, which varied in supply (number of sandwiches produced for the day) and demand (number of customers attracted). In Experiments…
Descriptors: Young Children, Supply and Demand, Inferences, Childrens Attitudes
Li, Pearl Han; Harris, Paul L.; Koenig, Melissa A. – Developmental Psychology, 2019
What does it take know a moral truth or principle? Although testimony is an undisputed source of empirical knowledge of contingent facts, it is less clear whether it is possible to acquire "second-hand moral knowledge" (Jones, 1999; Wolff, 1998). In the present studies, 3- to 5-year-old Chinese (N = 124) and U.S. American (N = 90)…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Comparative Education, Cultural Differences, Decision Making