Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 2 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3 |
Descriptor
Evaluative Thinking | 3 |
Age Differences | 1 |
Attribution Theory | 1 |
Beliefs | 1 |
Children | 1 |
Childrens Attitudes | 1 |
Christianity | 1 |
Cognitive Development | 1 |
Credibility | 1 |
Elementary School Students | 1 |
Experiments | 1 |
More ▼ |
Author
Corriveau, Kathleen H. | 3 |
Harris, Paul L. | 2 |
Clegg, Jennifer M. | 1 |
Cui, Yixin Kelly | 1 |
Davoodi, Telli | 1 |
Heiphetz, Larisa | 1 |
Kurkul, Katelyn E. | 1 |
Payir, Ayse | 1 |
Yan, Eleanor Fang | 1 |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 3 |
Reports - Research | 3 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
China | 1 |
North Carolina | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Payir, Ayse; Heiphetz, Larisa; Harris, Paul L.; Corriveau, Kathleen H. – Developmental Psychology, 2022
Recent research has shown that a religious upbringing renders children receptive to ordinarily impossible outcomes, but the underlying mechanism for this effect remains unclear. Exposure to religious teachings might alter children's basic understanding of causality. Alternatively, religious exposure might only affect children's religious…
Descriptors: Children, Religious Factors, Religious Education, Cognitive Development
Cui, Yixin Kelly; Clegg, Jennifer M.; Yan, Eleanor Fang; Davoodi, Telli; Harris, Paul L.; Corriveau, Kathleen H. – Developmental Psychology, 2020
When learning about the existence of unobservable scientific phenomena such as germs or religious phenomena such as God, children are receptive to the testimony of other people. Research in Western cultures has shown that by 5 to 6 years of age, children--like adults--are confident about the existence of both scientific and religious phenomena. We…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Childrens Attitudes, Parent Attitudes, Beliefs
Corriveau, Kathleen H.; Kurkul, Katelyn E. – Child Development, 2014
These two studies explored 3- and 5-year-olds' evaluation of noncircular and circular explanations, and their use of such explanations to determine informant credibility. Although 5-year-olds demonstrated a selective preference for noncircular over circular explanations (Experiment 1: Long Explanations; Experiment 2: Short Explanations),…
Descriptors: Young Children, Thinking Skills, Preferences, Evaluative Thinking