Descriptor
Age Differences | 15 |
Cognitive Processes | 15 |
Intelligence Differences | 15 |
Exceptional Child Research | 5 |
Sex Differences | 5 |
Higher Education | 4 |
Intelligence | 4 |
Intelligence Tests | 4 |
Memory | 4 |
Mental Retardation | 4 |
Test Validity | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Intelligence | 2 |
American Journal of Mental… | 1 |
CORE: Collected Original… | 1 |
Child Abuse & Neglect: The… | 1 |
Educational and Psychological… | 1 |
Author
Holzman, Thomas G. | 2 |
Beer, Jessica | 1 |
Benbow, Camilla Persson | 1 |
Calfee, Robert C. | 1 |
Cherkes, Miriam G. | 1 |
Cohen, Ronald L. | 1 |
Crowther, E. M. | 1 |
Fradin, Sasha | 1 |
Gallagher, Marie S. | 1 |
Geddie, Lane | 1 |
Greenberger, Ellen | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Research | 8 |
Journal Articles | 5 |
Speeches/Meeting Papers | 2 |
Non-Print Media | 1 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 1 |
Education Level
Audience
Location
United Kingdom (Great Britain) | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Cognitive Abilities Test | 1 |
Wechsler Intelligence Scale… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Calfee, Robert C. – 1969
Studies of recall and recognition short-term memory (STM) were reviewed, and a series of studies of serial recognition memory of normal and retarded children was described. In experiments using a recall procedure there were decrements in initial performance level with decreasing age and IQ but less evidence that forgetting occurred at a faster…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Exceptional Child Research, Intelligence Differences

Cherkes, Miriam G. – American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1975
Examined were the effects of chronological age and mental age on the understanding of logical rules by four subject groups (10- to 14-years-old): 10 average IQ Ss, 10 learning disabled Ss, 10 older educable retarded Ss and 10 younger educable retarded Ss. (Author/CL)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Exceptional Child Research, Handicapped Children

Holzman, Thomas G.; And Others – 1980
The cognitive determinants of number series completion performance were studied by presenting a systematic set of problems to adults and to children of high and average intelligence, grades 4 and 5. Pearson correlations between these problems and the number series on the Cognitive Abilities Test were .51, .78 and .43 for the average intelligence,…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Arithmetic, Cognitive Processes, Computer Assisted Testing
Seitz, Sue; And Others – 1968
Twenty high mental age (MA) subjects and 33 low MA subjects who had been in a free word association test 30 months previously were retested with the Moran 80-Word List. At the time of the previous testing, subjects in the high group had a mean chronological age (CA) of 17.1 and mean MA of 11.5; subjects in the low group had a mean CA of 15.3 and…
Descriptors: Abstract Reasoning, Age Differences, Association Measures, Child Development

Geddie, Lane; Fradin, Sasha; Beer, Jessica – Child Abuse & Neglect: The International Journal, 2000
Fifty-six children (ages 43 to 83 months) participated in an event conducted by two undergraduates dressed as clowns. Ten days later, interviews found metamemory ability, intellectual functioning, and temperament were helpful in determining a child's capacity to accurately recall information, although for the most part age was the best predictor.…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Child Abuse, Cognitive Processes, Higher Education

Morrison, James A.; Michael, William B. – Educational and Psychological Measurement, 1984
The concurrent and discriminant validity of La Prueba de Analisis Auditivo, a Spanish auditory perception test, and the validity of the perceptual deficit hypothesis and of hypotheses derived from verbal processing theory were evaluated in a sample of 114 second-grade Hispanic pupils receiving reading instruction in Spanish. (Author/BW)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Auditory Perception, Cognitive Processes, Encoding (Psychology)

Benbow, Camilla Persson; And Others – Intelligence, 1983
Students of high intellectual ability and their parents were tested on a battery of cognitive tests. Vernon's model of intelligence best fit results. A verbal-educational and a practical-spatial-mechanical factor explained most performance variance. Among children, age related to verbal but not spatial or mechanical abilities. (Author/RD)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Tests
Jensen, Arthur R.; Osborne, R. Travis – 1979
Longitudinal data on the auditory forward and backward digit span (FDS and BDS) subtests of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) were obtained at five age levels (between 6 and 13), in samples of white and black children. Factor analysis and analysis of variance of the data were conducted to test 5 hypotheses, related to Jensen's…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Black Students, Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Processes
Rohwer, William D., Jr.; Levin, Joel R. – 1970
The major emphasis of this study is on the comparative validities of paired-associate learning tests and IQ tests in predicting reading achievement. The study engages in a brief review of earlier research in order to examine the validity of two assumptions--that the construction and/or the use of a tactic that simplifies a learning task is one of…
Descriptors: Academic Achievement, Academic Education, Age Differences, Cognitive Processes
Gallagher, Marie S. – 1968
To investigate the interrelationships between creativity and seven variables, 74 students in grades 4 through 8 plus the vocational group at a school for the deaf were tested. In addition to creativity, subjects were tested for intelligence, academic achievement, imaginative productions, perception by teachers, self-evaluation career aspirations,…
Descriptors: Academic Achievement, Age Differences, Career Choice, Cognitive Processes
Greenberger, Ellen; And Others – 1970
Problem solving flexibility (PSF), an ability commonly assessed in creativity batteries, was studied in a sample of middle class children (grades 1 through 3, average IQ 114), tested on questions resembling Guilford's consequences procedure. An hypothesis linking PSF with alertness to and interest in the environment was generally supported, more…
Descriptors: Academic Achievement, Achievement Need, Age Differences, Anxiety

Cohen, Ronald L.; Nealson, Judi – Intelligence, 1979
Retarded subjects were compared with mental- and chronological-age matched controls on serial short-term memory (STM) tasks. Retarded subjects were inferior to the control groups on both primacy and recency items, under two recall conditions. These data are discussed in relation to possible mechanisms underlying IQ-related individual differences…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Elementary Secondary Education, Individual Differences
Holzman, Thomas G.; And Others – 1980
The cognitive determinants of number analogy performance were studied by systematically manipulating the processing demands imposed by the items. To explore sources of developmental differences in analogical reasoning, subjects were included from two age levels, grades 4 and 5 and college. To allow the investigation of individual differences in…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Arithmetic, Cognitive Processes, College Students
Williams, Charlotte L.; Tillman, M. H. – 1968
The effects of age and intelligence levels upon word associations were studied in 96 intellectually retarded, normal, and superior children with IQ's of 65 to 80, 91 to 110, and 117 to 158 respectively. A word association and a word usage task (reliability coefficients of .91 and .98) called for homogeneous responses to six form classes--count…
Descriptors: Abstract Reasoning, Adjectives, Adverbs, Age Differences
Crowther, E. M. – CORE: Collected Original Resources in Education, 1978
Adolescents, ages 11-12 and 13-14, were tested for maturity of judgment about stability-change situations and about time perspective. (CP)
Descriptors: Adolescents, Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes