Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 3 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 19 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 49 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 87 |
Descriptor
Associative Learning | 99 |
Memory | 39 |
Cues | 37 |
Foreign Countries | 34 |
Task Analysis | 32 |
Undergraduate Students | 32 |
Cognitive Processes | 28 |
Recall (Psychology) | 27 |
Statistical Analysis | 24 |
Semantics | 22 |
Reaction Time | 21 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 99 |
Author
Livesey, Evan J. | 4 |
Hutchison, Keith A. | 3 |
Balota, David A. | 2 |
Beesley, Tom | 2 |
Camos, Valérie | 2 |
Coane, Jennifer H. | 2 |
De Houwer, Jan | 2 |
Freedberg, Michael | 2 |
Gilbert, Rebecca A. | 2 |
Harris, Justin A. | 2 |
Hazeltine, Eliot | 2 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 99 |
Reports - Research | 88 |
Reports - Evaluative | 7 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Information Analyses | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 53 |
Postsecondary Education | 38 |
Adult Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
Australia | 7 |
United Kingdom | 6 |
Germany | 5 |
Indiana | 3 |
Belgium | 2 |
California | 2 |
Canada | 2 |
Iowa | 2 |
Missouri | 2 |
New York | 2 |
Switzerland | 2 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Stroop Color Word Test | 2 |
Wechsler Adult Intelligence… | 2 |
Wide Range Achievement Test | 2 |
Peabody Picture Vocabulary… | 1 |
Raven Advanced Progressive… | 1 |
Raven Progressive Matrices | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Toppino, Thomas C.; Cohen, Michael S. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2010
What do learners do when they control whether to engage in massed or spaced practice? According to theories by Son (2004) and by Metcalfe and Kornell (2005), the tendency for learners to choose spaced practice over massed practice should decline as item difficulty becomes greater. Support originally was obtained when pairs containing unfamiliar…
Descriptors: Metacognition, Difficulty Level, Learning Processes, Prediction
Barrett, Louise C.; Livesey, Evan J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2010
Perruchet, Cleeremans, and Destrebecqz (2006) reported a striking dissociation between trends in the conscious expectancy of an event and the speed of a response that is cued by that event. They argued that this indicates the operation of independent processes in human associative learning. However, there remains a strong possibility that this…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Reaction Time, Associative Learning, Learning Processes
Livesey, Evan J.; Harris, Irina M.; Harris, Justin A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2009
Participants in 2 experiments performed 2 simultaneous tasks: one, a dual-target detection task within a rapid sequence of target and distractor letters; the other, a cued reaction time task requiring participants to make a cued left-right response immediately after each letter sequence. Under these rapid visual presentation conditions, it is…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Associative Learning, Experiments, Task Analysis
Proctor, Robert W.; Yamaguchi, Motonori; Zhang, Yanmin; Vu, Kim-Phuong L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2009
Associations between corresponding stimulus-response locations are often characterized as over learned, producing automatic activation. However, 84 practice trials with an incompatible mapping eliminate the benefit for spatial correspondence in a transfer Simon task, where stimulus location is irrelevant. The authors examined whether transfer…
Descriptors: Semantics, Attention Control, Reaction TIme, Spatial Ability
Pacton, Sebastien; Perruchet, Pierre – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2008
In 5 experiments, results showed that when participants are faced with materials embedding relations between both adjacent and nonadjacent elements, they learn exclusively the type of relations they had to actively process in order to meet the task demands, irrespective of the spatial contiguity of the paired elements. These results are consonant…
Descriptors: Statistical Analysis, Cognitive Processes, Task Analysis, Attention
Mitchell, Chris; Nash, Scott; Hall, Geoffrey – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2008
A robust finding in humans and animals is that intermixed exposure to 2 similar stimuli (AX/BX) results in better discriminability of those stimuli on test than does exposure to 2 equally similar stimuli in 2 separate blocks (CX_DX)--the intermixed-blocked effect. This intermixed-blocked effect may be an example of the superiority of spaced over…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Associative Learning, Learning Theories, Males
Maki, William S. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2007
Ratings of the degree of association between words are linearly related to normed associative strengths, but the intercept is high, and the slope is shallow (the judgments of associative memory [JAM] function). Two experiments included manipulations intended to decrease the intercept and increase the slope. Discrimination training on many pairs…
Descriptors: Memory, Associative Learning, Bias, Association (Psychology)
Ramponi, Cristina; Richardson-Klavehn, Alan; Gardiner, John M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2007
The authors investigated depth-of-processing effects on conceptual priming by comparing incidental (implicit) and intentional (explicit) tests of word association. In Experiment 1, depth of processing at study influenced priming of weak and medium associates but not of strong associates. In Experiment 2, depth of processing influenced priming of…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Associative Learning, Auditory Stimuli, Visual Stimuli
McCabe, David P.; Balota, David A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2007
Three experiments are reported examining the effect of context on remember-know judgments. In Experiments 1 and 2, medium-frequency words were intermixed with high-frequency or low-frequency words at study or at test, respectively. Remember responses were greater for medium-frequency targets when they were studied or tested among high-frequency,…
Descriptors: Heuristics, Context Effect, Incidence, Word Recognition
Hicks, Jason L.; Starns, Jeffery J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2006
The authors tested source memory across three conditions, one in which 3 strongly associated primes of a target word were presented in the same source as the target, one in which primes were presented in a different source than the target, and one in which no associates of targets were encoded. In the first 2 experiments, target source memory…
Descriptors: Models, Memory, Prediction, Experimental Psychology
Pashler, Harold; Cepeda, Nicholas J.; Wixted, John T.; Rohrer, Doug – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2005
Some researchers have suggested that although feedback may enhance performance during associative learning, it does so at the expense of later retention. To examine this issue, subjects (N = 258) learned Luganda-English word pairs. After 2 initial exposures to the materials, subjects were tested on each item several times, with the presence and…
Descriptors: Feedback, Associative Learning, Second Language Instruction, Retention (Psychology)

Runquist, Willard N. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 1982
The relationship between cue differentiation and interference was investigated in three paired-associate recall experiments. It was concluded that discriminative coding is sufficient to eliminate interference resulting from cue confusion during associative learning, but that discriminative coding is not a necessary consequence of associative…
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Cues, Higher Education, Inhibition
McDaniel, Mark A.; Guynn, Melissa J.; Einstein, Gilles O.; Breneiser, Jennifer – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2004
Several theories of event-based prospective memory were evaluated in 3 experiments. The results depended on the association between the target event and the intended action. For associated target-action pairs (a) preexposure of nontargets did not reduce prospective memory, (b) divided attention did not reduce prospective memory, (c) prospective…
Descriptors: Memory, Cues, Associative Learning, Cognitive Processes
Rickard, Timothy C.; Bajic, Daniel – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2005
Despite earlier evidence that the presence of 2 redundant cues can facilitate activation of a common response, T. C. Rickard and D. Bajic (2004) found no dual-cue facilitation in the case of cued recall, provided that each cue-response association was learned independently. In this study the authors investigated the generality of their results…
Descriptors: Recall (Psychology), Cues, Cognitive Processes, Responses
Perruchet, Pierre; Cleeremans, Axel; Destrebecqz, Arnaud – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2006
After repeated associations between two events, E1 and E2, responses to E2 can be facilitated either because participants consciously expect E2 to occur after E1 or because E1 automatically activates the response to E2, or because of both. In this article, the authors report on 4 experiments designed to pit the influence of these 2 factors against…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Influences, Expectation, Associative Learning