NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 15 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Carneiro, Paula; Fernandez, Angel – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
Two experiments were conducted to examine whether children of different ages differ in their ability to reject associative false memories with the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm. Two different types of manipulations that are thought to facilitate false memory rejection in adults--slowing the presentation rate and issuing explicit…
Descriptors: Models, Editing, Age Differences, Rejection (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Havy, Melanie; Bertoncini, Josiane; Nazzi, Thierry – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
Consonants and vowels have been shown to play different relative roles in different processes, including retrieving known words from pseudowords during adulthood or simultaneously learning two phonetically similar pseudowords during infancy or toddlerhood. The current study explores the extent to which French-speaking 3- to 5-year-olds exhibit a…
Descriptors: Phonetics, Vowels, Task Analysis, Vocabulary Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Curtin, Suzanne – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
In this study, we examined the nature of infants' representations of newly encountered word forms. Using a word-object association task, we taught 14-month-olds novel three-syllable words differing in segments and stress patterns. At test, we manipulated the stress pattern of the word or the position of the stressed syllable in the word. Our…
Descriptors: Language Patterns, Syllables, Infants, Intonation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Morra, Sergio; Camba, Roberta – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2009
The goal of this study was to investigate which working memory and long-term memory components predict vocabulary learning. We used a nonword learning paradigm in which 8- to 10-year-olds learned picture-nonword pairs. The nonwords varied in length (two vs. four syllables) and phonology (native sounding vs. including one Russian phoneme). Short,…
Descriptors: Phonology, Associative Learning, Short Term Memory, Learning Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Martin-Chang, Sandra Lyn; Levy, Betty Ann; O'Neil, Sara – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2007
Successful reading instruction entails not only acquiring new words but also remembering them after training has finished and accessing their word-specific representations when they are encountered in new text. We report two studies demonstrating that acquisition, retention, and transfer of unfamiliar words were affected differentially by isolated…
Descriptors: Word Recognition, Retention (Psychology), Reading Instruction, Associative Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Kee, Daniel W.; Davies, Leslie – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1990
Mental effort expended during the creation of sentence elaborations by fifth graders performing two simultaneous tasks was greater for inaccessible than accessible pairs. The difference was not observed when subjects were provided with pair members in sentences. (RH)
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Difficulty Level, Elementary Education, Elementary School Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Davidson, Denise; Tell, Dina – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2005
In two experiments, the use of mutual exclusivity in the naming of whole objects was examined in monolingual and bilingual 3- and 6-year-olds. Once an object has a known name, then via principles of mutual exclusivity it is often assumed that a new name given to the object must refer to some part, substance, or other property of the object.…
Descriptors: Monolingualism, Bilingualism, Language Acquisition, Vocabulary Skills
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Ackerman, Brian P. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1986
Describes four experiments that examined the ability of second- and fifth-grade children and college adults to use "extra-list" cues to retrieve episodic information from memory. Shows that effective cue use varied with both the "match" of cue and event classification, and with the associative structure of permanent memory.…
Descriptors: Adults, Associative Learning, Classification, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Spelke, Elizabeth S. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1981
Infants viewed two objects that bounced in synchrony with two percussion sounds in order to learn about the relationships between sound and object. Learning was revealed in two ways: in a search test and in a transfer test. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Auditory Stimuli, Infants, Observational Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Needham, Amy – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2001
Investigated in 6 experiments how 4.5-month-old infants' perception of a display is affected by an immediate prior experience with an object similar to part of the test display. Found that infants' use of a prior experience is disrupted by changes in the features of the object, but not by change in its spatial orientation. (JPB)
Descriptors: Association (Psychology), Associative Learning, Cognitive Development, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Barr, Rachel; Vieira, Aurora; Rovee-Collier, Carolyn – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2001
Two experiments examined whether associating an imitation task with an operant task affected 6-month-olds' memory for either task. Results indicated that infants successfully imitated a puppet's action for up to 2 weeks only if the associated operant task (pressing a lever to activate a miniature train) was retrieved first. Follow-up study…
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Cognitive Development, Imitation, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Callanan, Maureen A.; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1994
Contrary to the basic-level restraint hypothesis, which suggests that young children learn basic-level words more easily than specific-level words, these five studies show that although other constraints on word meaning may be in place, children are flexible in interpreting new words at different hierarchical levels. (MDM)
Descriptors: Adults, Associative Learning, Childhood Attitudes, Classification
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Kee, Daniel W. Davies, Leslie – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1991
Examined third and seventh graders during dual tasks to estimate the mental effort required to elaborate accessible noun pairs. Findings indicated that the accessibility difference was limited to the third grade. Findings support a knowledge-base view of developmental differences in elaboration effort. (SH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Associative Learning, Child Development, Cognitive Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Thompson, G. Brian; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1996
Distinguished experimentally between the learner's use of independent grapheme-phoneme correspondences and determined whether in the initial year of reading instruction sublexical relations can be formed. Results could not be given alternative explanations by the developmental bypass hypothesis nor by accounts which predict exclusive use of onset…
Descriptors: Associative Learning, Child Development, Cognitive Development, Developmental Stages
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Beuhring, Trisha; Kee, Daniel W. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1987
Two experiments examine the relationships among metamemory knowledge, the use of associative memory strategies such as elaboration, and cued-recall memory. Results show that metamemory development predicted most of the grade difference in cued recall, suggesting that metamemory development may explain improvements in retrieval strategies as well.…
Descriptors: Analysis of Variance, Associative Learning, Cognitive Ability, Cognitive Development