Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 4 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 8 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 9 |
Descriptor
Age Differences | 9 |
Emotional Response | 6 |
Gender Differences | 4 |
Psychological Patterns | 4 |
Racial Differences | 4 |
Correlation | 3 |
Empathy | 3 |
Ethnicity | 3 |
High School Students | 3 |
Screening Tests | 3 |
Socioeconomic Status | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Grantee Submission | 9 |
Author
Claudia M. Haase | 3 |
Emily F. Hittner | 2 |
Furlong, Michael J. | 2 |
Bradshaw, Catherine P. | 1 |
Carter, D. | 1 |
Clary, Laura K. | 1 |
Corinne A. Holmes | 1 |
David B. Rompilla Jr. | 1 |
Deborah J. Wu | 1 |
Dowdy, E. | 1 |
Dowdy, Erin | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Reports - Research | 9 |
Journal Articles | 3 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 1 |
Education Level
High Schools | 3 |
Secondary Education | 3 |
Junior High Schools | 1 |
Middle Schools | 1 |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Center for Epidemiologic… | 2 |
Positive and Negative Affect… | 2 |
Beck Anxiety Inventory | 1 |
Locke Wallace Marital… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Emily F. Hittner; Claudia M. Haase – Grantee Submission, 2021
The present laboratory-based study investigated socioeconomic status (SES) as a moderator of the association between empathic accuracy and well-being among married couples from diverse socioeconomic backgrounds. Empathic accuracy was measured using a performance-based measure of empathic accuracy for one's spouse's negative emotions during a…
Descriptors: Empathy, Socioeconomic Status, Accuracy, Well Being
Jacquelyn E. Stephens; David B. Rompilla Jr.; Emily F. Hittner; Vijay A. Mittal; Claudia M. Haase – Grantee Submission, 2023
When confronted with an emotion prototype (e.g., loss), individuals may experience not only target emotions (e.g., sadness), but also nontarget emotions (emotions that are atypical or incongruent with an emotion prototype; e.g., gratitude in response to loss). What are the cognitive correlates of nontarget emotions? Drawing from models of emotion…
Descriptors: Executive Function, Correlation, Emotional Response, Short Term Memory
Homer, Bruce D.; Plass, Jan L.; Rose, Maya C.; MacNamara, Andrew; Pawar, Shashank; Ober, Teresa M. – Grantee Submission, 2019
"Executive function" (EF), critical for many developmental outcomes, emerge in childhood and continue developing into early adulthood (Blakemore & Choudhury, 2006). During adolescence there are important developments in "Hot EF," which involves using EF in emotionally salient contexts (Zelazo & Carlson, 2012). The…
Descriptors: Executive Function, Cognitive Ability, Age Differences, Prior Learning
Deborah J. Wu; Ryan C. Svoboda; Katherine K. Bae; Claudia M. Haase – Grantee Submission, 2021
The current laboratory-based study examined individual differences in sadness coherence (i.e., coherence between objectively coded sad facial expressions and heart rate in response to a sad film clip) and associations with dispositional affect (i.e., positive and negative affect, extraversion, neuroticism) and age in a sample of younger and older…
Descriptors: Individual Differences, Nonverbal Communication, Personality Traits, Neurosis
Waasdorp, Tracy Evian; Fu, Rui; Clary, Laura K.; Bradshaw, Catherine P. – Grantee Submission, 2022
Bullying bystanders' reactions are important for either stopping or perpetuating bullying behaviors. Given school-based bullying programs' focus on bystanders, understanding the associations between school-level factors and individual bystander responses can improve intervention efficacy. Data from 64,670 adolescents were used to examine bullying…
Descriptors: Educational Environment, Bullying, Middle School Students, High School Students
Jamie J. Jirout; Corinne A. Holmes; Kizzann Ashana Ramsook; Nora S. Newcombe – Grantee Submission, 2018
Spatial skills are consistently linked to mathematical reasoning, and are sensitive to intervention. One important spatial skill is spatial scaling. We evaluated whether (1) a playful scaling game might promote learning by providing feedback during play, and (2) spatial scaling is related to number-line estimation based on the mutual reliance on…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Feedback (Response), Educational Games, Scaling
Furlong, Michael J.; Nylund-Gibson, K.; Dowdy, E.; Wagle, R.; Hinton, T.; Carter, D. – Grantee Submission, 2020
Our UC Santa Barbara research team engages in ongoing efforts to enhance and validate the Social Emotional Health Surveys (Primary, Secondary, and Higher Education). The initiative to enhance the secondary version is supported by an Institute of Education Sciences grant (#R305A160157, 2016-2020), which provides funding to refine, standardize, and…
Descriptors: Surveys, Psychometrics, Test Validity, Mental Health
Furlong, Michael J.; Dowdy, Erin; Nylund-Gibson, Karen – Grantee Submission, 2018
This manual reports on the development and validation of the original Social Emotional Health Survey-Secondary (carried out between 2012 and 2017). We shared the first version of the SEHS-S because it had sufficient validation evidence based on research completed by 2015; hence, the form reported on in this manual is called the SEHS-S (2015)…
Descriptors: Surveys, Psychometrics, Test Validity, Mental Health
Reed, Sarah R.; Stahmer, Aubyn C.; Suhrheinrich, Jessica; Schreibman, Laura – Grantee Submission, 2013
Stimulus overselectivity is widely accepted as a stimulus control abnormality in autism spectrum disorders and subsets of other populations. Previous research has demonstrated a link between both chronological and mental age and overselectivity in typical development. However, the age at which children are developmentally ready to respond to…
Descriptors: Autism, Children, Stimuli, Pervasive Developmental Disorders