NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 8 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Puspitawati, Ira; Jebrane, Ahmed; Vinter, Annie – Child Development, 2014
This study investigated the spatial analysis of tactile hierarchical patterns in 110 early-blind children aged 6-8 to 16-18 years, as compared to 90 blindfolded sighted children, in a naming and haptic drawing task. The results revealed that regardless of visual status, young children predominantly produced local responses in both tasks, whereas…
Descriptors: Blindness, Cognitive Processes, Child Development, Naming
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Vinter, Annie; Fernandes, Viviane; Orlandi, Oriana; Morgan, Pascal – Research in Developmental Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2012
The aim of the present study was to compare the types of exploratory procedures employed by children when exploring bidimensional tactile patterns and correlate the use of these procedures with the children's shape drawing performance. 18 early blind children, 20 children with low vision and 24 age-matched blindfolded sighted children aged…
Descriptors: Blindness, Visual Impairments, Comparative Analysis, Tactual Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Vinter, Annie; Puspitawati, Ira; Witt, Arnaud – Developmental Psychology, 2010
Two experiments were reported that aimed at investigating the development of spatial analysis of hierarchical patterns in children between 3 and 9 years of age. A total of 108 children participated in the drawing experiment, and 224 children were tested in a force-choice similarity judgment task. In both tasks, participants were exposed to…
Descriptors: Recognition (Psychology), Experimental Psychology, Children, Investigations
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Picard, Delphine; Vinter, Annie – Child Development, 2007
The present experiments were aimed at testing Karmiloff-Smith's (1992) assumption that representational flexibility in drawing behavior requires the relaxation of a sequential constraint. A total of two hundred and forty 5- to 9-year-old children produced cross-category drawings (e.g., a house with wings) in 4 conditions. The results indicated…
Descriptors: Freehand Drawing, Children, Child Development, Sequential Approach
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Picard, Delphine; Vinter, Annie – British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2006
This study aimed at specifying the content of the representational redescription (RR) process assumed by Karmiloff-Smith (1992) with respect to the emergence of inter-representational flexibility in children's drawing behaviour. We hypothesized that the RR process included part-whole decomposition processes that are essential to the ability to…
Descriptors: Children, Freehand Drawing, Child Behavior, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Vinter, Annie – Child Development, 1999
Solicited 6- to 10-year olds' and adults' perceptually ambiguous drawings to which two different meanings could be attributed. Analyzed movement sequences to determine whether movements were modified in ways determined by the model's meaning. Found that drawing was sensitive to meaning at all ages. Sensitivity differed as a function of the model…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Ambiguity, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Vinter, Annie; Perruchet, Pierre – Child Development, 2000
Examined implicit learning in 432 four- to 10-year-olds in 3 experiments, using a new paradigm based on drawing behavior. Found that children modified drawing behavior following specially devised practice in such a way that the changes could not be viewed as resulting from deliberate adaptive strategies, with modifications lasting for at least 1…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Freehand Drawing, Learning Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Picard, Delphine; Vinter, Annie – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2005
We investigated the nature of graphic formulas in 5-, 7-, and 9-year-old children when they were asked to draw a house and a television in a free condition, and then to draw from photographs of these objects. Assuming that the frequency of occurrence of a feature in children's drawings reflects its semantic weight, we studied the relations between…
Descriptors: Freehand Drawing, Childrens Art, Semantics, Geometric Concepts